Macroscopic insights from mechanistic ecological network models in a data void Yangchen Lin 8 April 2015 Sidney Sussex College, University of Cambridge A dissertation submitted for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy 在缺乏数据的状态下以机械性生态网模型吸取宏观灼见林扬尘博士学位论文剑桥大 to Man Contents prefatory note iii summary v 1 exposition 1 1.1 A complex systems approach to ecology . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 1.2 The network paradigm in ecology . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 1.3 Research direction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4 1.3.1 Modeling strategy . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4 1.3.2 The data void . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 8 2 stochastic bioenergetic network model 13 2.1 Network construction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 13 2.2 Multispecies dynamics . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 19 2.3 Environmental noise . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 24 3 colour and synchrony of environmental noise 29 3.1 Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 29 3.2 Methods . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 35 3.3 Results . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 37 3.4 Discussion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 40 3.5 Appendix . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 47 3.5.1 Effects of non-predator-prey interactions . . . . . . . . . 47 3.5.2 Effects of noise colour and synchrony . . . . . . . . . . . 50 4 extinction and invasion do not add up 55 4.1 Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 55 4.2 Methods . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 59 4.3 Results . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 67 4.4 Discussion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 78 4.5 Appendix . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 83 4.5.1 Regression trees . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 83 4.5.2 Effects of community structure . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 90 4.5.3 Effects of extinctor dominance . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 91 i 5 collective effect of interaction modifications 93 5.1 Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 93 5.2 Methods . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 96 5.3 Results . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 99 5.4 Discussion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 105 5.5 Appendix . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 110 5.5.1 Supplementary figures . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 110 5.5.2 Plots of unpooled data . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 113 6 species decline in compartmentalized networks 123 6.1 Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 123 6.2 Methods . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 126 6.3 Results . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 129 6.4 Discussion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 135 6.5 Conclusion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 138 6.6 Appendix . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 140 6.6.1 Regression trees . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 140 6.6.2 Plots from all treatments . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 143 7 coda 163 literature cited 167 On the cover: the arms of the University of Cambridge, gules on a cross ermine between four lions passant gardant a book gules (granted 1573). ii prefatory note This work attempts to serve as a dissertation for a degree qualification, but even more importantly it has emerged symphonically from a voyage of exploration and discovery of the world and of myself that involved not only scientific research but also the concurrent pursuit of artistic and spiritual fulfilment. I am indebted to my supervisor, Professor William J. Sutherland, who accompanied and guided me on this exhilarating journey. I am also grateful to Dr. Andrea Manica and Dr. Rufus Johnstone, who helped man the proverbial Apollo mission control ensuring that the trajectory of my voyage into the unknown remained within survivable constraints. There are also others from all over the world who engaged me in thought-provoking discussions, seeded me with important ideas, and taught me skills that facilitated the execution of my plan: Stefano Allesina, Tatsuya Amano, Jordi Bascompte, Francesco Battocchio, Tim Benton, Edouard Bul- teau, Mark Burgman, Yohay Carmel, Tharsila Carranza, Charlotte Chang, Zhi Yuan Chor, Damon Civin, David Coomes, Bo Dalsgaard, Lynn Dicks, Marcin Dziubiński, Anders Eriksson, Lucas Faria, Laurens Geffert, Paulo Guimarães Jr., Hsun-Yi Hsieh, Martin Hughes, Sonia Kéfi, Krzysztof Kozak, Philip Krammer, Clinton Leach, Thomas Lewinsohn, Owen Lewis, Nick Maclaren, Simon Martin, Jane Memmott, Mark Novak, Jose Manuel Ochoa- Quintero, Steven Orzack, Amael Paillex, Kelvin Peh, Owen Petchey, Mathias Pires, Philip Platts, Paulo Inácio Prado, Dan Reuman, Lasse Ruokolainen, Jane Shevtsov, Marius Somveille, Phillip Staniczenko, Daniel Stouffer, Si Tang, Elisa Thébault, Antonio Tironi, Kristian Trøjelsgaard, Marco van der Leij, Weigang Yan, Justin Yeakel and Veronica Zamora. Furthermore, I am grateful to the many anonymous peer reviewers who scrutinized my submit- ted and resubmitted manuscripts in detail and provided invaluable feedback. And not least my graduate tutors Jillaine Seymour, Berry Groisman, Julius Ross and Ian Black, the college and department administrators Suzannah Horner, Angela Parr-Burman, Stephanie Prior, Linda Wheatley and Alice Jago, and my compatriots at the Conservation Science Laboratory, Depart- iii ment of Zoology and Sidney Sussex College, all of whom conspired to create a genial and inspiring environment. My scientific endeavours have also benefited from generous research and travel grants from the São Paulo Research Foundation (fapesp), South Amer- ican Institute for Fundamental Research, Nanyang Technological University, Sidney Sussex College, nerc Centre for Ecology and Hydrology and the British Ecological Society. Parts of this dissertation were published [1–4] but have been brought up to date and improved in the light of subsequent ideas and literature. Insights from here have also been contributed to coauthored publications [5, 6]. Declaration. This dissertation is the result of my own work and includes nothing which is the outcome of work done in collaboration except as declared in the preface and specified in the text. It is not substantially the same as any that I have submitted, or is being concurrently submitted, for a degree or diploma or other qualification at the University of Cambridge or any other university or similar institution except as declared in the preface and specified in the text. I further state that no substantial part of my dissertation has already been submitted, or is being concurrently submitted, for any such degree, diploma or other qualification at the University of Cambridge or any other university of similar institution except as declared in the preface and specified in the text. The dissertation does not exceed the prescribed word limit for the relevant Degree Committee. © Yangchen Lin Cambridge, spring 2015 iv summary Complexity science has come into the limelight in recent years as the scien- tific community begins to grapple with higher-order natural phenomena that cannot be fully explained via the behaviour of components at lower levels of organization. Network modeling and analysis, being a powerful tool that can capture the interconnections that embody complex behaviour, has there- fore been at the forefront of complexity science. In ecology, the network paradigm is relatively young and there remain limitations in many ecological network studies, such as modeling only one type of species interaction at a time, lack of realistic network structure, or non-inclusion of community dynamics and environmental stochasticity. I introduce bioenergetic network models that bring together for the first time many of the fundamental structures and mechanisms of species interactions present in real ecological communities. I then use these models to address some outstanding questions that are relevant to understanding ecological networks at the systems level rather than at the level of subsets of interactions. Firstly, I find that realis- tic red-shifted environmental noise, and synchrony of species responses to noise, are associated with increased variability in ecosystem properties, with implications for predictive ecological modeling which usually assumes white noise. Next, I look at simultaneous species extinction and invasion, finding that as their individual impacts increase, their combined impact becomes decreasingly additive. In addition, the greater the impact of extinction or invasion, the lesser their reversibility via reintroduction or eradication of the species in question. For modifications of pairwise species interactions by third-party species, a phenomenon that has so far been studied one interaction at a time, I find that the many interaction modifications that occur concurrently in a community can collectively have systematic effects on total biomass and species evenness. Finally, examining a higher level of organization in the form of compartmentalized networks, I find that the relationship between intercompartment connectivity and the impacts of species decline depends considerably on network topology and whether the consumer-resource functional response is prey- or ratio-dependent. Over- v all, the results vary considerably across model communities with different parameterizations, underscoring the contingency and context dependence of nature that scientists and policy makers alike should no longer ignore. This work hopes to contribute to a growing multidisciplinary understand- ing, appreciation and management of complex systems that is fundamentally transforming the modern world and giving us insights on how to live more harmoniously within our environment. vi 1 exposition 1.1 a complex systems approach to ecology The twenty-first century has been marked by the advent of a new frontier in humanity’s scientific enterprise: the so-called complexity science. It arose from the recognition that many locally interacting agents can give rise to a system with unexpected macroscopic properties, and that this system furthermore adapts to changes in its external environment over time and is often far from equilibrium, its trajectory highly dependent on conditions early in its history. Consequently, such systems are not amenable to mathe- matical analysis, and their future possible states are ‘not finitely prestatable’ [7]. Despite their real-world prevalence in the guise of financial markets, ecosystems, ant colonies and many others, their interesting and potentially beneficial or destructive behaviours are hardly understood or controllable. In his seminal monograph in 1997, Prigogine [8] called for scientists to move on from traditional deterministic approaches and embrace the indeterminism of complex systems. I think the next century will be the century of complexity. Stephen Hawking, San Jose Mercury News (2000) In ecology, it was realized as early as 1994 that research had (and as of today, has) been dominated by the approach of studying at most a few species or interactions at a time [9], inherited from the early days of observation and description of living organisms. This has partly been wrought by the difficulty of collecting detailed field data on more than a few selected components of the system (see §1.3.2), and partly by the popularity of simple mathematical models for finding analytical solutions of ecological equilibria. While exact, tidy and easy to interpret, the reductionistic approach does not encapsulate the complex interactions of nature [9]. Reductionism may be beginning to take its toll on advancement in ecological research. In a 2014 meta-analysis of more than 18 000 published articles, Low-Décarie et al. [10] 1 Exposition observed that despite an increasing number of hypotheses being tested per article in ecology, their explanatory power seemed to be declining. One possible cause cited was that the simple questions had mostly been answered, leading ecologists to tackle increasingly complicated questions. Although correspondingly sophisticated statistical procedures were used to attack such questions, it is not unreasonable to suppose that even those procedures will have difficulty explaining and predicting ecological behaviour as long as only a few components of the system are analyzed for extrapolation to the system as a whole. A more fundamental shift from reductionistic to holistic approaches may be needed to bring about the next golden age in ecological research [11]. 1.2 the network paradigm in ecology Network analysis is a powerful framework that has been applied to complex nonlinear systems in many disciplines [12], being flexible to the idiosyncrasies of each. It has been hailed as the tool for addressing the ‘big questions of contemporary science’ [13]. The versatility of the network approach for diverse purposes is illustrated, for example, by Schich et al. [14], who traced the evolution of cultural history via networks of intellectual mobility reconstructed from birth and death location data of individuals who had significant impacts on culture. Solé & Valverde [15] introduced a classification of the structure of many different types of networks, from the Internet to metabolic maps to food webs to brain networks, according to three axes: randomness, modularity and heterogeneity. In this classification, food web structure has moderately high randomness, low modularity and moderate heterogeneity and lies out- side the domain of ‘scale-free-like networks’, while ecological mutualistic networks have low randomness, moderately high modularity and moder- ate heterogeneity and lie within the domain of ‘scale-free-like networks’. Proulx et al. [16] reviewed the rising use of network analysis in various subfields of ecology and evolution and suggested that the time was ripe for advancing beyond pairwise interactions and building a ‘predictive science of biological networks’. More specifically to ecology, Bascompte [17] advo- 2 Yangchen Lin cated a network approach to the vital task of understanding how ecological interactions affect the emergent properties of complex ecosystems under in- creasing anthropogenic threat, while Brose [18] and Lewinsohn & Cagnolo [19] highlighted the value of ecological networks for understanding and predicting the consequences of species loss on ecosystems. Real ecological networks are highly complex, containing much omnivory and trophic looping [20]. Many studies of ‘networks’ and ‘food webs’, however, categorize the nodes into distinct trophic levels, and the inter- level links only connect adjacent levels. Moreover, such studies usually deal with a small number (2–3) of trophic levels [21]. It is important to account for vertical (trophic) diversity, as has been shown in the diversity-functioning debate [22]; the network approach is powerful in accommodating both horizontal and vertical (trophic) diversity [23]. Across the multidisciplinary network research landscape, ecological net- works are becoming recognized for their contribution towards the under- standing of general laws governing the properties of complex networks. Chiu & Westveld [24] demonstrated the application of social network analysis to food webs, while Brummitt et al. [25] drew parallels between power grids and ecological networks. Even more significantly, analogies between ecolog- ical and economic networks are getting increasing attention. Simple models of networks of bank loans have been built to demonstrate shock propa- gation through network structure, with conceptual comparisons to species extinctions in ecosystems [26, 27], and the power-law (scale-free) degree1 distribution tendency of both economic and ecological networks has been found to make them more susceptible to catastrophic collapse [28]. Stability measures for financial networks are also being developed that have potential applications to ecology [29]. Ecological network dynamics. Change over time is what really defines the interesting and important characteristics of ecological communities and com- plex systems in general, and is integral to any attempt to understand the impact of perturbation on ecological networks or make predictions for prac- tical management [16, 30–32]. The study of networks with temporal changes 1The number of nodes (species) to which a given node in the network is connected. 3 Exposition in information or energy flow between nodes is, however, still an emerging field in many areas of network science [33–37]. Ecology is no exception; much research on ecological networks has dealt with static networks, and there tends to be a disconnect between reductionistic dynamical studies of trophic modules versus static whole-network studies [38]. The importance of incorporating dynamics in whole-network studies has been underscored by the work of Eveleigh et al. [39], who found different outcomes in diversity cascades caused by outbreak species when realistic non-equilibrium dynamics were accounted for. Furthermore, most network-level dynamical ecological studies to date have dealt exclusively with food webs. Community-level conclusions from such studies may not hold true in real communities, where multiple non- predator-prey interaction types co-occur and interact with one another and with trophic interactions [40]. Forays into dynamical representations of multiple interaction types are still in their infancy [41]. For example, Melián et al. [42] incorporated dynamics for plant species in a community with multiple interaction types but assumed that animal population densities were constant. Blonder et al. [43] review the methodology for incorporating temporal dynamics into network analysis, and its application to networks in ecology and evolution. 1.3 research direction 1.3.1 modeling strategy The broad aims of this work are to investigate, using theoretical mod- els, how various interesting ecological phenomena at the mesoscale (species) level affect community-level behaviour and response to perturbation. I try to determine the presence or absence of general patterns that apply across different contexts, which addresses concerns about the predominance of spe- cific case studies in community ecology with very little general understanding [44]. Given the long-standing dichotomy between ‘simple mathematical mod- els’ and ‘large simulation models’ [45], it is essential to justify the simulation approach this research adopts. Ecological dynamics have traditionally been 4 Yangchen Lin understood via stability analysis of simple deterministic equations of a few interacting species [46–49], although community matrices of relatively large networks are increasingly being analyzed to elucidate the influence of net- work structure on interaction strengths [50] and the influence of both of these on stability [51]. Mathematical analysis continues to make valuable contributions to ecological network science, not least when allied with tech- niques such as model selection [52]. The so-called generalized modeling has also been promulgated [53, 54], which examines the dynamical stabilities of a large number of ecological network replicates with different model structures and parameter values and therefore avoids making simplifying assumptions. Like the other analytical approaches, however, it is largely focused on quantifying deterministic equilibrium stability. Simulation models, used in my study, are another powerful tool for un- derstanding ecological network behaviour [55], and complement mathemat- ical analysis [56], especially in nonequilibrium situations and where model complexity precludes analytical solutions. I depart from the emphasis on equilibrium stability, whether of traditional local equilibrium points in pop- ulation dynamics or of the latest ‘structural stability’ of ecological networks [57], and focus on tackling the confusion of a reality where nonequilibrium dynamics prevail [58, 59]. Indeed, in the latest overview of the continuing debate on model simplicity versus complexity, Evans et al. [60] reiterate that complex, mechanistic models can be better than simple models when seeking ecological generality, as well as when attempting specific prediction outside the boundary of current conditions [61, 62], and that the current preponderance of ecology on simple models may even be stifling ecological progress [63]. This is not to say that all models should be complex. We need to harness the complementary powers of both simple and complex models to give us the best overall understanding [60]. For example, efforts to isolate small trophic modules from theoretical or empirical food webs for the prediction of equilibrium responses to perturbations [64–66] should be matched by efforts that scale back up from modules to the larger eco- logical networks in which they are embedded [67], in order to capture all the feedback loops. 5 Exposition How does the oft-invoked complexity-stability relationship colour the dichotomy between simple and complex models? The relationship has been of central interest in ecology since the pioneering work of May [68, 69]. Despite significant advancements [58, 70–72], the issue of when and how ecological complexity stabilizes or destabilizes an ecosystem remains a hot topic of research and debate [73–75]. At the same time, deeper understanding of this relationship has become ever more relevant to scientists and policy makers hoping to mitigate potentially debilitating effects of anthropogenic pressures on ecosystems [74, 76]. My prognosis is that more complex ecolog- ical networks may be more resilient in overall function due to redundancy and degeneracy [77], but may also be more prone to unexpected endoge- nous behaviour caused by internal feedback loops and thresholds, making the complexity-stability relationship ill-defined. The purpose of my thesis, however, is not to leap into the fray of the complexity-stability debate which continues to receive ample attention. Instead, the complexity-stability issue serves here to highlight that simple and complex systems can behave differ- ently. Although simple models may capture some of the essential features of the complex system that exists in reality, they may not capture the rare but potentially important or catastrophic behaviour, or the unprestatable future states [7, 78] discussed earlier (§1.1). Conversely, and rarely considered in ecological research, relationships that exist within simple models might well be nonexistent in more complex systems with large numbers of alternate pathways. There are two main system-level simulation approaches to ecology that are of the level of complexity pertinent to this thesis: those of ecosystem ecology and community ecology. The ecosystem ecology approach [79–82] is an established system dynamics modeling methodology used mostly in marine ecology [83], where trophic flows of energy and nutrients between functional compartments in a particular system are specified and various indices of energy flow are calculated. More rarely, both trophic and nontrophic flows are included [84]. The ecosystem ecology approach has been used extensively for the analysis of aquatic ecosystems [85–88] and applied to the assessment 6 Yangchen Lin of environmental sustainability [89] and of the effectiveness of marine protected areas [90]. Aggregated biotic and abiotic compartments and flows, however, are perhaps not the most optimal design for studying the effects of species- species transactions. Moreover, the ‘positive effects’ sometimes referred to in ecosystem ecology mainly concern negative local interactions having net positive effects on network-level energetics [79, 91, 92], rather than facilita- tive interspecific interactions. This is where community ecology complements ecosystem ecology. Simulation modeling in community ecology is best repre- sented by bioenergetic modeling of consumer-resource dynamics [41, 93–95], which has recently acquired increased ecological realism via integration with models of ecological network structure [96] and with facilitative interactions. The bioenergetic approach, where species interactions are mechanistically parameterized in terms of energy and metabolism, is equally amenable to studying the response of complex ecosystems to environmental changes in- cluding those brought about by climate change (e.g. Gilbert et al. [97]). Current research in this area is moving towards a more applied flavour even if the actual modeling remains theoretical, such as a study of how food web diversity and modularity mediate the effects of hypothetical pollutants [98]. Olesen et al. [99] (p. 37) suggest that networks of species interactions may be no more than arbitrary structures constructed by scientists that do not exist in reality, but the authors do not elaborate. Insights into what this might mean can be found in the paradigm of agent-based modeling (abm). A strong case for abm is that ecological interactions really take place between individual organisms rather than between the non-physical aggrega- tions known as species. To be reasonably realistic, individual-level network models have to incorporate at least the two main classes of interactions: between conspecifics and between individuals of different species—perhaps this is the true ecological network that actually exists. Most existing abm studies, however, have dealt with only either conspecific [100] or interspe- cific [101] interactions at the individual level. An additional challenge is that abm can become very computationally intensive if many species are involved. Again, the kind of species-level model used in my study treads a reasonable 7 Exposition compromise between simplicity and complexity, while incorporating both in- terspecific and intraspecific interactions, the latter in a phenomenological yet realistically ‘individual’-inspired way via terms such as density dependence and cannibalism (§2.1). 1.3.2 the data void But there are also unknown unknowns—the ones we don’t know we don’t know. Donald Rumsfeld A discourse on modeling cannot be complete without some discussion of empirical data for model validation. Ecology continues to stand apart from many other sciences as a particularly data-deficient discipline. It is often impossible to collect comprehensive data on entire communities. Where this is attempted, data may be insufficient or biased and the model is usually restricted to one part of a system at one specific locality. Data shortage is particularly severe in the case of ecological networks, where it is extremely difficult to detect the presence of all the interspecific interactions, let alone measure interaction strengths. Many data sets of food web structure that are regarded as highly resolved are nevertheless incomplete [102], encompassing only a subset of taxonomic or ecological functional groups. For example, there is a ‘food web’ data set of over 1700 species that comprises only plants and herbivores [103], and a ‘predator-prey network’ data set of high temporal resolution over millennial time scales that comprises only mammals [104]. Most data sets also exclude parasites [105, 106], Preston et al. [107] being a rare exception. The system is often simply too diverse to sample comprehensively. For example, 156 ‘kinds of organisms’ (species, distinct life history stage of a species, or group of closely related species) are recorded in the data set from tropical forest at El Verde Field Station in Puerto Rico [108], but the actual number of species in tropical rainforests is widely estimated to be much higher. Large data sets from marine biomes are probably closer to the 8 Yangchen Lin actual number of species of the specific system [109–111], but there remains the aforementioned problem of imperfect observation of interactions. Wirta et al. [112] recommended combining multiple data sources, including field observations, lab experiments and molecular data, to gain better resolution of ecological networks and reduce bias. This, however, may still be insufficient for diverse systems due to the sheer number of unobserved species and an even greater number of their interactions. Besides, the structural properties of any one or a few comprehensively sampled food webs may not be representative of food webs in general, especially with so much contingency of food web structure [113] and other ecosystem characteristics [114]. There is another way in which data are incomplete: all the above examples have only predator-prey interactions. Any given ecosystem is an intricate web of co-occuring trophic and nontrophic interactions (see Chapter 3 for further discussion about combining them in research), but data on these different types of interactions are rarely collected sym- patrically. There are abundant trophic and nontrophic interaction data, such as the Interaction Web DataBase at the National Centre for Eco- logical Analysis and Synthesis, University of California at Santa Barbara (http://www.nceas.ucsb.edu/interactionweb), and calls continue for ecolo- gists to pool their data into collective databases in order to harness the power of big data [115]. Such data, however, are from different localities and cannot be aggregated to represent a single system. Empirical datasets on the network structure of multiple interaction types in the same system are rare, notable examples being those from an agroecosystem in Britain [116] and from Chilean rocky shores [117]. When one considers superimposing community dynamics on top of net- work structure, there arises a further problem, that of interaction strengths. Reliable specific predictions of complex ecological communities require highly accurate estimates of the interaction strengths [118], but interaction strengths are very difficult to measure accurately in the field [119], especially in terms of rates of energy flux with standardized units of measurement across species. For example, mutualistic interactions are often quantified as the number of pollinator visits to flowers, but the relationship of visit fre- 9 Exposition quency to actual nectar consumption or flower fertilization rate is unclear. Admittedly, more work is needed to develop the empirical domain of a research topic that has been dominated by theoretical modeling [120]. Some advances have nevertheless been made in fitting mechanistic dy- namic models to empirical data of medium-sized networks of 24–50 nodes. Two studies [121, 122] found that when fitted to data on food web structure, observed biomass and body sizes, such models reproduced seasonal dynamics and community patterns (such as size-abundance distribution) relatively well once dynamical parameters were allometrically scaled. This can be useful where taxonomically comprehensive time series data for dynamical param- eterization are scarce [122]. These studies, however, involved only trophic interactions in specific systems; fitting a limited model to data from a specific locality may not be the most appropriate paradigm in my study searching for general patterns in communities with multiple interaction types. There is a popular culture in science that regards models as useless or even misleading for the real world unless they are validated by sufficient quantity and quality of data [123]. More complex models are naturally more susceptible to this criticism. It is indeed the case that policy and action informed by predictions from such a model are considered risky [124]. But I do not think this is the whole story. Even with abundant and quality data, a model may not make specific real-world predictions to the required accuracy for practical use, even with sensitivity analysis. In contrast, whether data are sufficient or not, a suite of models based on sensible, documented assumptions and comprising a range of plausible model structures and parameter values [125, 126] can provide valuable general insights and anticipation about how real systems might behave under given circumstances—as is the premise of this work. Indeed, this so-called Monte Carlo approach has been advocated even for predictive purposes, as in the case of complex climate models which have so far not been subject to stochastic parameterization [127]. In any case, researchers have advocated general insights rather than specific prediction of ecosystem responses to disturbance [128], and suggested that the appreciation of and preparedness for the variability of the future is as important as, if not more important 10 Yangchen Lin than, prediction [129]. The virtues of the general approach should all the more be embraced given that the ecosystem-level ‘data vacuum’ [124] cannot be filled before conservation needs to happen [130, 131]. In summary, my model tries to capture the essential features of the system by incorporating all the main classes of interaction types and dy- namics in a succinct way, producing a model that is ‘realistic yet simple’ [132]. Furthermore, it is grounded upon empirical data of network struc- ture and knowledge of the dynamics of ecological processes, and subject to Monte Carlo methods. Any model should be based on the foundations of all available knowledge and data on the system of interest [133]—it is the synergy across multiple sources of quantitative data and qualitative knowledge that allows a model to transcend artificial boundaries and tell us something of which we would otherwise be ignorant. At the same time, however, the model does not blindly adhere to these data and knowledge, since they are not necessarily omnipotent representations of reality (recall the discussion above about incomplete data). Nevertheless, the model serves as a springboard to explore the endless possibilities of the state space. Pat- ten [134] cautioned that too much of ecological research has been focused on empirical approaches for immediate local application, with too little ef- fort in advancing the foundational science and theory that is important for deep understanding, especially at the systems level. My study aims to contribute to addressing this imbalance, in the hope of facilitating future synergy between empirical and theoretical efforts in the science of ecology [135] and beyond. 11 2 stochastic bioenergetic network model This chapter is a documentation of the network simulation model used in this dissertation; see §1.3.1 for the philosophy and motivations behind the model. The architecture of the model involves the construction of a network of interacting species and the specification of allometrically scaled bioenergetic equations governing changes in species states over time [93–95]. The combination of structural models with bioenergetic dynamics has been suggested as a way towards greater ecological realism [96]. My model is among the first to extend the bioenergetic model to include nontrophic interaction types alongside trophic interactions (see Kéfi et al. [41]), and introduces various refinements for greater realism. Overall, it could be said to apply the principles of pattern-oriented modeling [136] by incorporating observed patterns in ecological networks that are important to the research question. Simulations and analyses were programmed in R [137] and C++ with Boost libraries for random number generation. In addition, the simulations for Chapter 6 were executed in parallel using Message Passing Interface on the Darwin supercomputer at the University of Cambridge. Numerical integration was performed using the fourth-order Runge-Kutta algorithm with dt = 0:1. 2.1 network construction A food web topology is generated using the niche model [138], which has been found to be one of the most robust so far in emulating the structure of real food webs using relatively simple rules [96, 139–142] and continues to be used in the latest research [41]. The model allows for the variety of complications observed in nature, such as trophic loops (rock- paper-scissors), omnivory, apparent competition and intraguild predation [143, 144], although no correction is made for the relative frequencies of occurrence of these topological ‘motifs’ (see Bascompte & Melián [144] and Stouffer et al. [145]). There have been various generalizations of the niche 13 Stochastic bioenergetic network model model [139, 142, 146–149]. Here a modified version of the generalization by Stouffer et al. [146] is combined with that by Warren et al. [148], as explained below. Unlike the original niche model, real food webs are not fully interval [146]. The method of Stouffer et al. [146] introduces an adjustable degree of diet discontiguity, with their parameter x fixed at 0:8, which closely matches empirical data [146]. One additional modification is made: if the upper bound of a predator’s original diet range exceeds the predator’s niche value, the so-called ∆k (Stouffer et al. p. 19017, last paragraph) number of species is selected from non-prey species of niche values smaller than or equal to the aforementioned upper bound rather than the predator’s niche value. This improvement, particular to my study, avoids the logical problems associated with reducing the upper bound of the potential range. More recent generalizations of the niche model [139, 142, 149] have achieved better fit to data than Stouffer et al. These versions, however, are less suited to the current application. It is difficult to specify the con- nectance in the formulation of Allesina et al. [139]. The models of Williams et al. [142, 149] do not assign parameter values using statistical distributions that would keep the model sufficiently simple. As my purpose is to con- struct multiple plausible food webs rather than predict the links of specific empirical webs as accurately as possible, the method of Stouffer et al. is adequate, even in the light of more biologically motivated models based on optimal foraging (see Petchey et al. [150] and skepticisms as to whether optimal foraging reflects reality [151, 152]). Contrary to Williams et al. [142], I am not overly concerned by the assumption that species near the centre of a predator’s diet range are not more likely to be prey than those near the edges, since unmodeled multidimensional niche space [149, 153]) could account for that. The one-dimensional niche axis I use does not preclude the effects of multidimensional niche space. The inverse niche model of Warren et al. [148] is used to optionally add parasite species to the food web. This institutes one of the most fundamental differences between predators and parasites, by ‘letting little things eat big things’ [105]. The inverse niche model excludes parasite- 14 Yangchen Lin parasite and predator-eats-parasite links, the connectance being defined as the proportion of possible host-parasite links that are realized [154]. Although predation on parasites is not included as direct links in the inverse niche model, it is manifested via predators consuming the hosts that harbour the parasites [148], which has been found to be the most common form of parasite ingestion in aquatic food webs [155]. Hyperparasitism and competition among parasites are not incorporated; these are avenues for future empirical and theoretical work. It has also been suggested that parasitism may be incorporated into food webs as indirect rather than direct interactions [156] (see §5.1). The connectances of the predator-prey and host-parasite subwebs are set at 0:15 [154, 157]. Connectance in real webs can vary by as much as an order of magnitude [158]; the value used here is a reasonable starting point close to the connectances of several large and highly resolved food webs [158]. The niche algorithms are run repeatedly until a web is produced that has subwebs of the desired connectances ±0:01 and that satisfies the criteria that the web must not contain any totally unconnected species, and that no heterotrophic species should consume only itself, since this is energetically unsustainable. In the qualifying web, all nonparasite species having no prey are desig- nated as basal species [94] flexibly representing autotrophs and detritivores, which are not distinguished. Species that consume prey could also qualify as detritivores or autotrophs, such as carnivorous plants, but these are ex- cluded for simplicity. Model realizations containing only one basal species are discarded, as direct competition among basal species (see below) cannot be implemented in these realizations. Random networks. For comparison with the niche models (Chapter 4 on- wards), models with random network topology and at least two basal species were also generated. The species niche values conformed to a uniform dis- tribution as in the niche model. The consumer-resource links were assigned in random pairs and directions regardless of niche values, with the condition that basal species could not be consumers. This implementation also implic- itly incorporates more trophic loops or parasitism, because a consumer can 15 Stochastic bioenergetic network model be of smaller body size (niche value) than its resource with a probability equal to the converse. Competition. Direct competition interactions, where implemented, were assigned randomly to 25% of the total number of potential interactions between all pairs of basal species, rounded up to the nearest whole num- ber. This procedure changes the overall connectance, but this is permissible because the previously published connectance values pertain only to their respective interaction types. The random assignment of competitive interac- tions gives rise to varying degrees of intransitivity [159], reflecting natural variability. Mutualism. For both niche and random networks, mutualistic interactions are assigned randomly to the predator-prey subweb, excluding species pairs already allocated to other interaction types, with a connectance of 0:15 of the total number of potential interactions between all pairs of nonpar- asite species, rounded up to the nearest whole number. This procedure allows for a wide variety of motifs including plant facilitation, predator facilitation [160], intraguild mutualism [5, 161, 162], plant-animal mutualism and three-way mutualism such as that among sea anemones, zooxanthellæ and anemonefish [163]. It also does not exclude less commonly studied mutualistic associations in ecological networks, such as herbivore-detritivore mutualisms e.g. gut microbes. Some of the even more esoteric mutualisms, however, are not yet implemented, such as vector-parasite mutualism [164]; these are again avenues for future inquiry. The non-implementation of the nestedness and modularity observed in bipartite mutualistic networks [165–167] is not considered problematic. This is because the overall structure of mutualistic interactions in the whole community is unlikely to be as highly nested or modular as that of many pollination- or seed dispersal-based bipartite mutualistic subwebs that have been observed in isolation, when species from the different subwebs interact with one another in other kinds of mutualism not encompassed by any of those subwebs, such as habitat provision2 in return for nutrients [163, 168]. 2Where habitat provision enhances protection of the occupant from particular predators, tripartite interaction modification (Chapter 5) is a more appropriate representation. 16 Yangchen Lin Table 2.1. Parameters of ecosystem assembly. Parameter Value Reference fraction of species that are parasites (:33 [154, 173] x (:0 [146] connectance (trophic subweb) (:)5±(:() [157] connectance (host-parasite subweb) (:)5±(:() [154] connectance (competition subweb) (:25 this paper connectance (mutualistic subweb) (:)5 this paper Furthermore, nestedness may not be a significant predictor of community persistence [169]. The mutualistic connectance used here is lower than the value of approximately 0:3 measured from bipartite mutualistic networks [170]. It makes sense to have a lower connectance for the full ecological network because its constituent species encompass a broader range of taxa than any one specialized bipartite network of taxa that rely heavily on mutualism. The overall concept of different types of binary interactions used here was first put forward by Burkholder [171] who listed nine ‘coactions between weak and strong organisms’ comprising different combinations of ‘+’, ‘−’ and ‘0’ denoting gain, loss, and neither gain nor loss by each of a pair of interacting species. This concept has been further developed by Fath & Patten [91] and Fath [92]. My model simplifies it by incorporating the three ‘elementary particles’ that are arguably the minimum set that represents the different types of interactions at the most basic level: +−, −−, ++. Finally, the complete ecological network (Fig. 2.1) is tested to ensure that there are no isolated components by computing the number of times 0 appears as an eigenvalue of the Laplacian, which is the number of components of the graph [172]. If isolated components have been produced, the network is regenerated until there is none. Network assembly parameters are summarized in Table 2.1. Fontaine et al. [174] were among the first to propose combining struc- tured subwebs of different interaction types; my network takes their concep- 17 Stochastic bioenergetic network model Predator axis Parasite species P re y o r h o st a x is predation cannibalism competition mutualism parasitism 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 Figure 2.1. One stochastic realization of the network construction algorithm com- bining the niche model of predator-prey interactions (columns 1–20), inverse niche model of host-parasite interactions (columns 21–30), direct competition and mutual- ism. The first 20 columns represent the same set of species as the 20 rows. Black boxes below the diagonal dashed line represent trophic loops. Columns with no predation are defined as basal species. Direct competition interactions are assigned randomly among basal species. The number of parasite species is set at half that of nonparasite species. Connectances: (:)5± (:() for niche and inverse niche models and mutualism. tual framework one step further in that the predator-prey and host-parasite subwebs are niche models rather than the more rudimentary nested or mod- ular structures. For computational tractability, the number of species in the networks used in this dissertation (10–30 species) is only moderately large. The results nevertheless remain relevant to complex ecosystems—trophic ecological networks of less than 14 nodes and 40 links have been found to behave similarly to larger networks due to thermodynamic and other mechanisms constraining food chain length and trophic complexity [175]. 18 Yangchen Lin 2.2 multispecies dynamics Network dynamics are modeled using allometrically scaled bioenergetic equations [93, 94] with various modifications. The dynamics of a given basal species i and a given non-basal species j with energy states Ei and Ej respectively in arbitrary units are described by the equations dEi dt = ri [ 1− Ei Ki ] Ei − i(1− ϵi)fiEi − ∑ k∈consumers k Φk;ifkEk  (2.1a) dEj dt = j ( −1 + ϵj + ∑ k∈resources Φj;k ) fjEj − ∑ k∈consumers k Φk;jfkEk  (2.1b) with time t, maximum consumption rate per unit metabolic rate [94], assimilation efficiency  [93], predator-prey functional responses Φ (see below), nontrophic functional response f (see below) and ϵ, the species response to multiplicative environmental stochasticity (§2.3). is assigned a value of 6 which is between 4 for ectotherm vertebrates and 8 for invertebrate predators [95].  is assigned a value of 0:65 which is between 0:45 for herbivores and 0:85 for carnivores [95, 125], since the model includes omnivory. It is assumed that parameter values for predator-prey and host- parasite relationships are similar. The traditional intrinsic growth rate r and mortality rate are given versatile interpretations. In this case, r represents both somatic and repro- ductive growth arising from any net exogenous abiotic input such as detritus, insolation or inorganic compounds, subtracting egestion and excretion. This takes the cue from Halnes et al. [147] to embrace detritus in the niche model, but does it in a more species-oriented way suited to the focus of this study. The number of parameters for multiple basal species is minimized by drawing r⃗ from lognormal distributions whose expected values and vari- ances are equal to ⃗−1=4, accounting for metabolic scaling where metabolism in plants is defined as photosynthesis instead of respiration [176, 177]. The lognormal distribution is a sensible choice because it produces positive and 19 Stochastic bioenergetic network model Table 2.2. Parameters of multispecies dynamics. Parameter Description Unit Constraints Reference  species niche value none ((; )) [138]  assimilation efficiency none [(:45; (:05] [93, 95, 125] r net exogenous input rates time−1 ((;∞) to basal species K carrying capacity arbitrary ((;∞) biomass net exogenous output rates time−1 ((;∞)  scaling coefficient for none this paper mutualism and competition maximum consumption rate none [93, 94] normalized to metabolic rate ! resource preference none ((; )) [95] h Hill exponent none [); 2] [94, 125] ‘Exogenous’ refers to all abiotic energy and matter. mostly moderate values, with allowance for some high values representing the occasional fast-growing species. The parameters ⃗ are coefficients of exogenous output in terms of both metabolism and mortality. Being related to body size [95, 178, 179], ⃗ are assigned the values 0:01⃗−1=4. There is no lognormal randomization here because doing so would give some species large mortality rates leading to rapid extinction; unlike for r, there is no (positive) density dependence implemented in the model to prevent this. The normalization constant [177] 0:01 is chosen to produce ecologically sensible outputs i.e. to minimize extinctions and prevent excessive segregation into high-energy basal species and low-energy non-basal species, while keeping species states mostly within (0; 1) and producing a long-tailed species biomass distribution at steady state, as observed in preliminary simulations. Environmental stochasticity is formulated to act on , as is a basic population parameter common to both autotrophic and heterotrophic species. Negative density dependence is enforced on each basal species, with its carrying capacity K = 1=4 where 0 <  < 1. This complies with the positive general relationship of total species biomass to body size [177]. The fact 20 Yangchen Lin that  is not perfectly correlated with trophic position, due to dependence on the species’ behavioural and life-history traits, is already manifested in the randomized assignments of diet midpoints and diet ranges in the niche model. The predator-prey functional responses are given by the fraction of consumer i’s maximum consumption rate allocated to resource j [94, 95], Φi;j = !i;jEj h 0:5h + ∑ k∈resources !i;kEk h (2.2a) h ∼ U [1; 2], (2.2b) where resource preference ! is kept uniform and constant as 1=(no¯ of re- source species), the half-saturation density, i.e. resource density at which consumption rate is half the maximal rate [93], is fixed at 0:5 [125], and the Hill exponent h is drawn from a uniform distribution in the range [1; 2] giving a continuum between Holling types ii and iii functional re- sponses [94, 125]. The half-saturation density is kept constant for simplicity; 0:5 is ecologically reasonable in this model because species energies are initialized on the scale of [0; 1]. Overall, this kind of formulation allows interaction strength to be modulated as a function of prey preference and half-saturation density. Ratio dependence. Most research on community dynamics, particularly bioen- ergetic modeling of food webs, has assumed prey dependence. Ratio de- pendence, a form of predator-prey functional response that depends on the abundances of both predator and prey, was introduced by Arditi & Ginzburg [180] in 1989. It has long been a matter of controversy [181] as to whether real communities are prey- or ratio-dependent; the ecologi- cal community now generally accepts that reality is somewhere in between [182]. Arditi & Ginzburg [183] review the latest empirical evidence-based arguments for ratio dependence. Specific studies on ratio dependence have mostly been restricted to single prey and predator species or aggregated functional groups [184, 185]. In a theoretical discourse with multiple prey 21 Stochastic bioenergetic network model and predator species, van den Berg [186] found that ratio dependence ex- plained the attenuation of energy fluxes of trophic cascades in food webs. Ratio-dependent functional responses have been highlighted as an important mechanism to account for in models [185], and as a promising avenue for future theoretical work [187]. The present study draws comparisons between exclusively prey-dependent (with no predator interference term) versus ex- clusively ratio-dependent functional responses, representing the two ends of the continuum in order to clearly reveal any effects of predator influence. My ratio-dependent functional response is adapted from Piana et al. [184], Φi;j = !i;j(Ej=Ei) h 1h + ∑ k∈resources !i;k(Ek=Ei) h (2.3a) h ∼ U [1; 2], (2.3b) where the half-saturation of the prey:predator ratio is given a value of 1 as an ecologically sensible first approximation. If the state of a predator is zero, the ratio for that predator is assigned a value of 0 to avoid division by zero. This is also ecologically sensible since it would give Φ = 0. Nontrophic interactions. The bioenergetics of nontrophic interactions are very rarely implemented compared to trophic interactions. Here, mutualism and/or competition act on for a given species i, according to fi = 1 + comp ∑ g∈competitors EiEg 1 + mut ∑ g∈mutualizers EiEg (2.4) where  controls the overall strength of mutualism or competition and is assigned a value of 1 in this study. In my model, mutualism reduces while competition augments (Eqns. 2.1); when mutualism and competition are absent, is unchanged. The functional response is linear; this is not an unreasonable assumption as the real shape of the functional response of mutualism is not yet generally known [41]. Although the formulation is more rudimentary than that suggested by Kéfi et al. [41] and assumes that 22 Yangchen Lin a given species benefits or suffers to the same degree with every interactor, it is a reasonable starting point given that data are unavailable. Parameter values for community dynamics are summarized in Table 2.2. My parameterization procedure is somewhat different from that of Brose et al. [95], who use basal growth rate as a starting point for defining the time scale and metabolically scaling the other parameters. Nevertheless, my parameter values are metabolically scaled and ecologically sensible, while slightly relaxing the metabolic scaling constraints to allow for species with unusual or extreme natural histories that occasionally occur in real ecosys- tems. Like Brose et al., in my study the time scale is not defined in absolute terms; the emphasis is on producing ecologically plausible simulated trajec- tories and on the simulation duration being long enough to capture the patterns of density dependence, species interactions and stochasticity, such that any general results found would be applicable to a wide variety of ecosystems operating at different time scales. At the start of the simulation, each species in both niche and random networks, including basal and parasite species, is assigned an initial state E0 =  1=4, with the same rationale as for carrying capacity K . Basal species are therefore at carrying capacity at the start of a simulation; their interac- tions may nevertheless bring them to equilibrium states different from their carrying capacities. In plausible extreme cases, we may have r(1−E=E0) < 0 or (−1+∑ Φ) S 0 when the population exceeds the carrying capacity or when resources are abundant, causing mortality to exceed growth or vice versa. No distinction is made between parasite and nonparasite species when assigning initial states; contrary to intuition, parasites in an ecosystem can sometimes have a total biomass equal to or greater than that of nonparasites [188]. The model has two other simplifications. Firstly, there are real-world cases where an interaction between two species is both trophic and mutu- alistic, for example in pollination; considering merely the net effect may obfuscate some aspects of the dynamics [161]. There is scope for more research in this area [41]. Secondly, the model does not implement adap- tive rewiring of network links [189] in a mechanistic way based on optimal 23 Stochastic bioenergetic network model foraging theory [190–192]. The model, however, does have intrinsic adaptive rewiring in both trophic and nontrophic interactions, via the dependence of interaction strength on species energy, although the values of the resource preference ! are kept fixed over time. It also accounts for the formation of new links if one interprets the links assigned to a given species at the beginning as including all the species it could potentially interact with under all scenarios. This has the merit of not assuming optimal foraging behaviour. The overall model specification strikes a compromise between simplicity and complexity, and avoids setting arbitrary and discrete thresholds at which to change links. 2.3 environmental noise This section documents the implementation of environmental noise in the models; see §3.1 for background. Code written by the author can be down- loaded at https://github.com/linyangchen/noise. The noise term ϵ is based on a time series c generated using the equations of Cohen et al. [193] given by ct = T 2∑ i=1 √( i 2.i )ϱ sin ( 2.i i t+ Ωi ) (2.5a) Ω ∼ U [0; 2.] (2.5b) where i is the number of cycles (frequency) within the total duration of c , i is the length of c , ϱ is the spectral exponent i.e. noise colour, and Ω is the phase shift. i is set to the length of the network model simulation, in which case the period of the shortest noise cycle is 2dt and the period of the longest cycle is equal to the simulation duration. Each sine curve can be interpreted as representing one environmental variable; summing multiple sine curves of different phases and periods creates the overall environment experienced by the species. 24 Yangchen Lin A m p li tu d e −1.5 −1.0 −0.5 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5 ∑ am p li tu d es −4 −2 0 2 4 6 Time (arbitrary units) Figure 2.2. Generation of coloured environmental noise of spectral exponent ϱ 5 ) using the spectral synthesis method [193] of sine wave summation (Eqn. 2.5a). The time series of noise terms ϵ is finally produced using spectral mimicry [194], permutating a Gaussian-distributed random sequence of mean 0 and specified variance such that it matches the spectral density of c . This gives a Gaussian-distributed colour noise series for a fair comparison with white noise cases [195], although it does not necessarily have to be Gaussian in reality [196]. This mechanistic noise implementation has advantages over the commonly used phenomenological autoregressive models [193], and does not underestimate extinction risk in red environments like both 1=f and autoregressive models [195]. There remains a limitation in the method; as ϱ ≥ 3, the noise increasingly looks like a smooth sine curve, which is not realistic (L. Ruokolainen pers. comm.). The total variance of the noise experienced by each species is fixed at 0:1 (see Sæther et al. [197]), with ϵ constituting all, half or none of this variance. In the second of these three different treatments, the remaining variance experienced by each species comprises values independently drawn from Gaussian distributions with means equal to ϵ⃗ and variance 0:05. In the third treatment, the variance experienced by each species over time comprises values independently drawn from Gaussian distributions with mean 0 and variance 0:1. These procedures produce different degrees of interspecific 25 Stochastic bioenergetic network model synchrony; partial synchrony represents the environment being either good or bad for most species in a given time step, but to different extents for each species. 26 a multispecies Pythagorean time series Anton Bruckner: Symphony no¯ 8 3 colour and synchrony of environmental noise affect network variability Abstract. I use stochastic bioenergetic ecological network models to explore the effects of non-predator-prey interactions, colour of environmental noise, and the degree of synchronization of environmental noise across species on the coefficients of variation over time of total ecosystem energy content and Shannon entropy. In regression trees derived by binary recursive partitioning, the presence or absence of synchrony gave the greatest difference in the means of data points for both system energy and Shannon entropy, followed in turn by white versus coloured noise and pink versus red and black noise. The effects are present despite the large variation in the Monte Carlo simulations reflecting the variability of real ecosystems. Non-predator-prey interactions explained relatively small proportions of the total deviance, and each had different directionalities of effects depending on the presence of the other interaction types and on whether energy or entropy was measured. These results underscore the importance of modeling more realistic colours of environmental noise in understanding and predicting the dynamics of ecological communities. 3.1 introduction God is noisy. Real ecosystems are ‘buffeted by a more or less continual series of perturbations, and transient behavior may be the norm rather than the exception in nature’ [198]. In the face of increasing environmental change, it is important to understand the impact of different types of environmental variation on populations and communities [199]. Sutherland et al. [200] identified the question of how environmental stochasticity interacts with density dependence to influence population dynamics as one of the most important contemporary questions in ecology. Stochasticity cannot be un- derstood adequately by linear analysis, because the stochastic system is never in equilibrium [201]. Indeed, Ruokolainen & Fowler [202] reported that 29 Colour & synchrony of environmental noise analytical solutions did not capture many of the features of the simulation outputs of stochastic Lotka-Volterra models. The biggest problem is the real world. md at Goldman Sachs The influence of stochasticity on population dynamics was first inves- tigated in the 1960s and 1970s [196, 203, 204] and have attracted increasing attention in recent years. Stochasticity has been found to magnify extinc- tion risk and reduce invasion risk [205, 206], and has been highlighted as an area for further research in community viability analysis [207]. With respect to species interactions, Ripa et al. [208] first presented a theory of the population-level effects of environmental noise in two-species ‘food webs’. A theoretical study [209] of the effects of noise on the transient dynamics of two, three and n interacting species with Lotka-Volterra dy- namics found that noise can enhance stability, but that analysis used random interaction parameters. Subsequently, Ripa & Ives [210] took an analytical approach to understanding the effects of environmental synchrony on the dynamics of populations in a two-species Lotka-Volterra competition model, and showed that the effects can be large and unexpected as synchrony can either amplify or dampen cyclic behaviour. Gravel et al. [211] investigated population-dynamical criteria for species coexistence in a stochastic envi- ronment, while Gjata et al. [212] used stochastic simulations to study the effects of indirect interactions resulting from trophic interactions. Vasseur & Fox [213], using a theoretical four-species ‘diamond’ food web, reported that noise can stabilize food webs by synchronizing population dynamics. Wells et al. [32] found that environmental stochasticity, on top of popula- tion dynamics, affected the structural indices of ecological networks. Most recently, Novak [187] highlighted nonequilibrium dynamics as one of the promising avenues for future theoretical work. Reddened noise is experienced by most natural populations [214–219] and is exhibited by many other complex systems ranging from stock markets to protein-dna binding to rhythmic synchronization in music-making [220], 30 Yangchen Lin yet most stochastic studies have assumed white noise. The colour of noise has been shown to have significant impacts on the dynamics and stability of single populations [221–228] or simple systems with a few interacting species [202, 206, 210, 229]; also see review by Ruokolainen et al. [199]. Multispecies studies include that of Greenman & Benton [206], who reported that au- toregressive noise caused resonance and threshold effects in a simple system of three species on three trophic levels. Using a two-species bioenergetic trophic model, Vasseur [229] showed that the temporal variability of each species can respond differently to changes in environmental noise colour. Ruokolainen et al. [230] and Ruokolainen & Fowler [202] constructed theoretical models showing how various factors affected the extinction risk of populations in competitive Lotka-Volterra communities exposed to dif- ferent environmental noise colours. The question remains, however, as to whether findings from studies of noise in single-species populations or a few interacting species would hold for networks of many interacting species. With multiple interacting species, the question also arises as to the extent to which individual species’ responses to stochasticity are correlated, or synchronized, with one another. Total interspecific asynchrony has often been used in studies of competition and niche differentiation [231], but this setup is unsuitable for predictive models because environmental forcing itself is one of the synchronizers of population dynamics for many taxa [232–236], especially among species with similar traits [233, 237]. Niche differentiation nevertheless gives rise to asynchrony, which can make diverse communities less variable and more stable in the face of environmental disturbances [238, 239]; partial synchrony would therefore seem consistent with most real communities. Very little is known about how the degree of synchrony affects the dynamics and stability of ecological networks, existing studies being limited to white noise and either triangular tritrophic food webs [240] or Lotka-Volterra competitive communities [241]. Nontrophic interactions. Recent studies have investigated the properties of networks of various non-predator-prey interaction types, one of which is competition. For example, species coexistence has been reported to depend on the mean and variance of interaction strengths in competitive networks 31 Colour & synchrony of environmental noise [242]. Competitive networks constructed on game theory principles have been found to promote diversity [243], which implies that this enables more species to coexist in equilibrium. In networks of competing species that cannot be ranked in a strict hierarchy of competitive ability, negative frequency dependence can arise that promotes diversity [159]. In contrast, Loreau & de Mazancourt [241] found in a stochastic competition model that competition was generally destabilizing, but this may have been partly due to the inherent instabilities of the discrete-time modelling that was used. Ecological theory has traditionally been dominated by research into antagonistic interactions with, until recently, a relative neglect of facilitative or mutualistic interactions [244]. Although the co-occurrence of negative and positive interactions was already recognized by Burkholder [171], only in recent years have researchers begun to more widely acknowledge the importance of accounting for the mutualistic interactions that pervade real ecological communities [161]. Mutualism is not only of theoretical interest but has also been shown to enhance sustainability in systems exploited by man [245] and has become more prevalent under increasing environmental stress [246]. Various studies have taken the network approach towards mutualistic interactions. For example, Suweis et al. [167] analytically and numerically demonstrated the positive correlation between species abundances and the nestedness of animal-plant mutualistic networks. Okuyama & Holland [247] found that the structural attributes of dynamic mutualistic networks give rise to positive complexity-resilience relationships, while Ramos-Jiliberto et al. [192] reported that incorporating adaptivity of interactions increased robustness in dynamic mutualistic networks. Discoveries about mutualistic networks have also inspired further inquiry into food webs. For example, Kondoh et al. [248] found that food webs had nested substructures like in bipartite mutualistic networks, and proposed that nestedness in food webs hinders coexistence because it increases consumer niche overlap, an effect opposite to nestedness in mutualistic networks. A comparative study by 32 Yangchen Lin Thébault & Fontaine [249] revealed how dynamic food webs and mutualistic networks achieve stability via different structural mechanisms. The aforementioned studies have greatly advanced our understanding of complexity-stability relationships in ecosystems, but one of their limitations is that interaction types were considered in isolation [132, 249, 250]; little is known about the combined effect of all these interaction types. The relative neglect of non-predator-prey interactions can have profound ramifications for our understanding of ecosystem function [251]. For example, non- predator-prey interactions may exacerbate human impact on ecosystems [252]; not accounting for such interactions in fisheries models has reduced the capacity of these models to predict stock collapse [82]. Food webs implicitly include some non-predator-prey interaction in the form of indirect competition, but competition among basal species and, more strikingly, facilitation are absent. Conversely, studying non-predator-prey interactions in isolation can also compromise our ability to make useful predictions. For example, the dynamics of host-parasite interactions depend not only on those interactions per se but also on predators and alternative hosts, with implications for infectious disease control [253]. Several researchers have thus advanced the design of models containing multiple interaction types. The analytical models of Gross [161], involving multiple consumer species feeding on a single resource, revealed that in- traguild mutualism could be an important ingredient for species coexistence in otherwise competitive environments. Filotas et al. [254] simulated how spatial processes affect the structure and stability of multiple-interaction- type networks created with random topology and link strengths. Most recently, the so-called multiplex network approach [255] has increasingly been adopted, where different interaction types are partitioned into differ- ent networks that are interlinked with one another [174, 256, 257]. Some of the most general results so far emerging from networks of multiple interaction types come from Allesina & Tang [51], whose ana- lytical models suggested that the addition of mutualistic and competitive interactions reduce the probability of stability of predator-prey networks. Stability of equilibrium points, however, is just one part of the story, as 33 Colour & synchrony of environmental noise first highlighted in §1.3.1; the response in nature of a system to perturba- tions constitute another important line of inquiry. Indeed, some researchers have advocated that the maintenance of resilience, rather than avoidance of disturbance, should be the focus for conservation efforts [258, 259]. Host-parasite interactions. The importance of incorporating parasites into mainstream food webs [260] and potential effects of parasitism on food web stability [154, 261] have also been highlighted recently, as most studies have investigated host-parasite networks in isolation [262, 263]. There have even been calls for including parasites in food webs by ‘default’ [264]. The impacts of parasites on food webs have been found to be diverse, various interspecific and intraspecific mechanisms having been reported for both stabilizing and destabilizing effects [105, 156, 265, 266] and for effects on network structure mainly via changes in diversity and complexity [267]. Parasitism has also been paired with mutualism in a study that explored the dynamics and equilibria of a theoretical ‘food web module’ comprising a plant, pollinator and nectar robber [268]. To date, no study has investigated the effects of the colour and syn- chrony of noise on networks of many species and multiple interaction types. Most of this kind of work has been on the dynamics of single populations; even when interspecific interactions are considered, often us- ing Lotka-Volterra models of predation or competition, the focus is on population-level rather than ecosystem-level dynamics. In this study, I use the simulation model documented in Chapter 2 to ask how environmen- tal stochasticity and synchrony, and non-predator-prey interactions, affect the variability of ecological networks. The foundations of the model in- clude empirical data from which network structures have been derived, and well-established consumer-resource equations with known assumptions. Pre- liminary simulations using this class of model by Kéfi et al. [41], comprising food webs with added plant facilitation and predator interference, indi- cated that nontrophic interactions result in higher diversity at equilibrium, but their study did not look at environmental stochasticity. I show that the colour and degree of synchrony of noise have striking effects on the temporal variability of ecological networks. 34 Yangchen Lin 3.2 methods The models are based on Chapter 2, using the prey-dependent functional responses. Williams & Purves [149] found no consistent trend across empir- ical food webs in the distribution of niche values within a given web and reported that the distributions were often nonuniform. In this chapter, I therefore modify the original niche model for greater realism by using beta- distributed ( = 1:5; = 5) niche values ⃗. Given that  in the niche model is roughly proportional to body size [269, 270], my values of and produce a distribution that approximates two relationships3: the proportionality of species diversity to (body size)−1=4 [177], and the lognormal distribution of body sizes as shown in studies of terrestrial and stream ecosystems [271]. As in the food web, I set  ∼ B(1:5; 5) in the parasite web, since species having parasitic or parasite-like lifestyles can have macroscopic (e.g. hyenas and avian brood parasites) as well as microscopic body sizes, and there is higher diversity at small body sizes as for nonparasitic species. Independent replicate 15-species models with different combinations of interaction types were generated. I used eight configurations: f, fc, fm, fcm, fp, fcp, fmp and fcmp, where f denotes food web (predator-prey), c competition among basal species, m mutualism and p parasitism. In the parasitic model configurations, five of the species were parasites; the pro- portion of parasites takes guidance from Lafferty et al. [154] and Sukhdeo [173]. Environmental noise was implemented as detailed in §2.3. ϱ was varied from 0 to 2 in increments of 1, 0 being white noise, and 1 and 2 being widely regarded as pink and red respectively. For each degree of synchrony (none, half, full—see §2.3) in each noise colour treatment in each model configuration, 100 independent network models were generated. The networks were simulated using the ranges of dynamical parameter values listed in Table 3.1. Temporal variability of the ecosystem was quantified in terms of the coefficients of variation (cv) over time [58, 272] of two metrics: the total system energy and the exponent of the Shannon entropy [273]. 3 In later chapters, species niche values are uniform rather than beta distributed, to facilitate comparison with random-topology models where body size hierarchy no longer applies, and to avoid making species of lower niche values that go extinct more similar to surviving species. 35 Colour & synchrony of environmental noise Table 3.1. Parameter values of multispecies dynamics. Parameter Value Reference  B():5; 5) this study  (:25 [276] r E(r) 5 −1=4 [177] K E0 this study (:()−1=4 [177]  ) this study 6 [93] ! )=(no¯ of resource spp.) [95] h U[); 2] [125] The cv combines the amplitude and frequency of temporal variation in one convenient index. The total system energy or biomass at each time step was obtained by summing the species states. The exponent of the Shannon entropy measures the directly comparable effective number of species [273], given by exp ( −∑Si=1 pi ln pi) where pi is the proportion of system energy constituted by species i at a given time step and h is the total number of species. The metrics were measured from time step 10 001 (to exclude initial transients)4 for a duration of 25 000 divisions. Preliminary simulations indicated that this time scale of measurement encompassed all the main features and periodicities of the stochastic behaviour. The effects of noise colour and synchrony on ecosystem temporal vari- ability were analyzed using regression trees grown using binary recursive partitioning using the tree package [275] in R. Regression trees were also used to examine the effects of dummy variables representing the presence or absence of each non-predator-prey interaction. The advantages of re- gression trees over regression models are that the former is nonparametric and does not assume unimodality and linearity of the response variable. 4 A possible question for future work is how transient dynamics modulate stochasticity and vice versa, as has been done for single-species plant populations [274]. 36 Yangchen Lin 3.3 results The results reported here are from a rerun of the entire simulation study using a different pseudorandom number generator seed state from Lin & Sutherland [1] and with noise for basal species implemented in mortality rate rather than growth rate, which represents a minor advancement in model design since publication. While the results for non-predator-prey interactions are slightly different (as would be expected and, as emphasized by Lin & Sutherland [1], to be interpreted with caution given the very small effect sizes), those for the effects of noise are qualitatively unchanged, reaffirming them. In a small number of simulations, numerical integration produced neg- ative species states when dE=dt < 0 and |dE=dt| S E. These were due to the noise terms and the small errors inherent in numerical integration; the affected simulations were omitted from the analysis. The total sample sizes for the various model configurations are 894 (f), 894 (fc), 893 (fcm), 897 (fcmp), 896 (fcp), 893 (fm), 895 (fmp) and 895 (fp), of 900 simulations each. The stopping criterion for growing the regression trees was set as the point where the within-node deviance became less than 1% of the root node deviance. The resulting trees are shown in Fig. 3.1 and numerically summarized in Table 3.2. The split between unsynchronized and synchronized (half and full) species environmental responses explains most of the deviance, with considerable differences in the mean values of the cv of system energy (0:0381 and 0:307 respectively) and exponent of Shannon entropy (0:0319 and 0:145 respectively). The next most important factor is noise colour, with white noise giving lower cv than coloured noise for system energy (0:0463 and 0:437 respectively), and white or pink giving lower cv than red noise for entropy (0:0884 and 0:258 respectively). The effects of synchrony were much more pronounced when the noise was coloured (Fig. 3.3). The non-predator-prey interaction types never explained more than 1% of the deviance when the dummy variables representing their presence or absence were analyzed together with the noise variables. 37 Colour & synchrony of environmental noise envar = {0} fpow = {0} fpow = {1} 0:21770 0:03805 0:30720 0:04627 0:43690 0:32350 0:55040 Energy envar = {0} fpow = {0; 1} fpow = {0} 0:10740 0:03186 0:14490 0:08842 0:03656 0:13990 0:25770 Entropy Figure 3.1. Regression trees of effects of noise colour and synchrony on cv of system energy (top) and of ]pp[Shannon entropy] (bottom). Explanatory variables: envar, noise synchrony; fpow, colour (denoted by spectral exponent). The categorical explanatory variable levels partitioned to the left-hand branches are indicated. Numbers under nodes are mean values of the response variables apportioned to the respective branches above. Regression trees using only dummy variables of non-predator-prey interaction types, with data points pooled across different colours and degrees of synchrony of noise, are summarized in Fig. 3.2 and Table 3.3. Mutualism explains most of the deviance in the cv of system energy, while parasitism explains most of the deviance in the cv of the exponent of Shannon entropy. Both have stabilizing effects i.e. reduce system variability. Competition generally explains the smallest proportion of the deviances. 38 Yangchen Lin Table 3.2. Regression tree nodes and deviances of effects of noise colour and synchrony on the cv of total system energy or ]pp[Shannon entropy]. Tree Node n Deviance Mean cv energy envar 7)57 402 (:2)0 terminal 2371 )7:3 (:(30) fpow 4770 35( (:3(7 terminal )506 )(:3 (:(463 fpow 3)12 )70 (:437 terminal )517 34:7 (:323 terminal )515 )(2 (:55 entropy envar 7)57 )36 (:)(7 terminal 2371 5:11 (:(3)1 fpow 4770 )(1 (:)45 fpow 3)03 3):4 (:(004 terminal )506 4:34 (:(366 terminal )517 )0:6 (:)4 terminal )515 47:6 (:250 Tree and node names refer to Fig. 3.1; n, number of data points. Regression trees were also grown separately for each noise colour- synchrony combination (Appendix 3.5.1). The results were similar to the aforementioned effects pooled across noise colours and degrees of syn- chrony. Mutualism explained most of the deviance most of the time for the cv of both energy and entropy, often causing considerable if not greatest increase in node purity. Where they account for most of the deviance, mutualism and parasitism both tend to have stabilizing effects, except in the case of unsynchronized species responses to noise, where mutualism tends to be destabilizing. The effects of direct basal competi- tion are again inconsistent and relatively small. The effects of mutualism and parasitism, where they do not account for most of the deviance, are also relatively small and inconsistent. 39 Colour & synchrony of environmental noise M = {0} P = {0} C = {0} C = {0} P = {0} C = {0} C = {0} 0:2177 0:2342 0:2426 0:2415 0:2438 0:2259 0:2324 0:2194 0:2012 0:1967 0:1976 0:1958 0:2057 0:2070 0:2045 Energy P = {0} C = {0} M = {0} M = {0} M = {0} C = {0} C = {0} 0:10740 0:11250 0:10990 0:11140 0:10830 0:11520 0:11640 0:11400 0:10220 0:10730 0:10960 0:10510 0:09704 0:09778 0:09630 Entropy Figure 3.2. Regression trees of effects of non-predator-prey interactions on cv of system energy (top) and of ]pp[Shannon entropy] (bottom), pooled across noise colours and degrees of synchrony. Explanatory variables: C, direct basal competition; M, mutualism; P, parasitism. The categorical explanatory variable levels partitioned to the left-hand branches are indicated (( and ) respectively denote absence and presence of the interaction type). Numbers under nodes are mean values of the response variables apportioned to the respective branches above. 3.4 discussion Models with coloured noise, but not white noise, exhibit considerably larger and longer-term fluctuations when species environments are partially or fully synchronized. This is most likely due to constructive interference with density-dependent dynamics within [224] and across populations, arising from temporal autocorrelation and synchrony of the environment. These large fluctuations may mimic periodic outbreaks and crashes of 40 Yangchen Lin Table 3.3. Regression tree nodes and deviances of effects of non-predator-prey interactions on the cv of total system energy or ]pp[Shannon entropy]. Tree Node n Deviance Mean cv energy M 7)57 402 (:2)0 P 3571 20) (:234 C )700 )57 (:243 terminal 014 77 (:242 terminal 014 71:0 (:244 C )71) )24 (:226 terminal 015 65:2 (:232 terminal 016 50:7 (:2)1 P 3570 2(( (:2() C )706 00:) (:)17 terminal 013 44:3 (:)10 terminal 013 43:0 (:)16 C )712 ))) (:2(6 terminal 015 57 (:2(7 terminal 017 54:3 (:2(5 entropy P 7)57 )36 (:)(7 C 3574 77 (:))3 M )707 36:) (:)) terminal 014 2(:1 (:))) terminal 013 )5:2 (:)(0 M )707 4(:1 (:))5 terminal 014 24:3 (:))6 terminal 013 )6:6 (:))4 M 3503 50:6 (:)(2 C )71) 3):6 (:)(7 terminal 015 )6:6 (:)) terminal 016 )5 (:)(5 C )712 26:1 (:(17 terminal 015 )4:7 (:(170 terminal 017 )2:2 (:(163 Tree and node names refer to Fig. 3.2; n, number of data points. species in real systems,5 something that my white-noise and/or unsyn- chronized models do not appear to represent adequately. These species 5Or even bubbles and crashes in the economy. 41 Colour & synchrony of environmental noise 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5 Configuration FCMP a 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5 System energy exp[Shannon entropy] b 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5 0 1 2 Noise colour (spectral exponent) c C V o f ec os y st em m et ri c ov er t im e Figure 3.3. The influence of noise colour and degree of interspecific synchrony on variability of ecosystem energy and Shannon entropy, in terms of kernel density distributions of outputs from multiple simulations (n ≈ 1((), for model configuration fcmp. Plots for the other model configurations are qualitatively similar (Appendix 3.5.2). a, unsynchronized species environments; b, half synchronized; c, fully synchronized. Violins scaled to constant width. may be native species, invasive pests or viruses, the latter two of which would be of direct concern to human welfare and development. It is also notable that with white noise, synchrony did not destabilize systems more than the unsynchronized case. This could be because of the weakness of reinforcing effects due to the absence of temporal autocorrelation in white noise, whether synchronized or not. Although Lögdberg & Wen- nergren [228] found that environmental reddening reduced rather than increased species extinction risk, the focus of their model on the spatial environment experienced by a single species was different from mine. My 42 Yangchen Lin results generally concur with the findings of Greenman & Benton [224] for single populations subject to coloured versus white noise. Stochastic food web models that assume white noise may give the idea that ecosys- tems are less variable, less prone to large and sudden changes and more predictable than they really are, with implications for the reliability and efficiency of ecological and conservation projects that do not conduct research, monitoring, data collection or management over sufficiently long time scales. There was more than a two-fold difference in both measures of ecosystem variability between simulations with synchronized compared to unsynchronized noise. These large differences highlight the importance of accounting for synchrony in ecological modeling and prediction. A multispecies model that implements stochasticity independently for each species may underestimate instability and extinction risk, especially if those models also assume white noise. That synchrony has relatively little influence on ecosystem variability in white-noise scenarios, but that even partial synchrony amplifies the effect of coloured noise (or vice versa) in increasing ecosystem variability, underscores the risks of making multiple simplifying assumptions in nonlinear ecological modeling in general and the need to minimize such risks by striking the best possible compromise between simplicity and realism of models. Modeling noise as realistically as possible is especially crucial in fields of research such as critical transitions in complex systems (see Scheffer et al. [277]), where stochasticity plays a key role in the dynamics before, during and after critical transitions. I recommend pink noise [216, 218] with partial synchrony as a starting point for a more realistic representation of environmental stochasticity in dynamic species interaction models. In the literature, direct competition among basal species rarely appears outside exclusively plant-based research. Some studies [278–280] have taken the leap by combining direct basal-species competition and plant- herbivore interactions in bitrophic designs, but have not examined the effects of such competition on a web of many trophic levels and multiple interaction types, including facilitation. My full webs indicate that the 43 Colour & synchrony of environmental noise effects of direct basal competition depend on whether parasitism or mutualism, or both, are also present. Competition, being an antagonistic interaction, may stabilize systems containing facilitation (mutualism) by dampening the enhanced growth rates brought about by mutualism (see below), and amplify the variability of systems containing parasitism— another antagonistic interaction—by depressing species abundances further and thereby causing larger abundance fluctuations and knock-on effects through multispecies interactions. Continuing along this line of argument, the very small effects of competition on systems containing both mutualism and parasitism could be because the opposing effects of competition in the presence of either mutualism or parasitism alone balance out each other when both mutualism and parasitism are present. Although the ‘connectance’ of direct competition (0:25) seems higher than those of the other interaction types, the absolute number of competition links is relatively small because of the small number of basal species. We can expect any effects of direct competition to become clearer with increasing connectance. I did not, however, increase the connectance further because it is more important in the systems-oriented approach here to have a sensible balance between different interaction types. In any case, there is little basis for comparing the relative effect sizes between different interaction types. The finding that mutualism tends to stabilize the system could be attributed to the tendency of mutualism to dampen predator-prey cycles. The occurrence of both negative and positive effects of mutualism in this study, however, indicates that the influence of mutualism could be highly context dependent. Nevertheless, the results complement the findings of Mougi & Kondoh [281] who found that when increasing proportions of randomly selected antagonistic interactions in a model food web were converted6 to mutualistic interactions, intermediate amounts of 6 A potential limitation with their methodology is that the conversion of antagonistic interac- tions to mutualistic interactions meant that food web structure could not be kept constant in their study. 44 Yangchen Lin such mutualistic interactions generally had stabilizing effects in terms of equilibrium stability based on the Jacobian community matrix. In my model, the key difference between having and not having parasite species is the difference in the prevalence of species of smaller body size gaining energy at the expense of species of larger body size. This allowed me to examine the effect of this difference while keeping the other attributes of the parasite species, such as assimilation efficiency, distribution of niche values and metabolic scaling, statistically similar to those of the nonparasite species. Nevertheless, the model assumptions and parameterizations been made with nonparasite species in mind may not necessarily apply to parasites [105]. More investigation is needed on how parasites differ topologically and dynamically from nonparasites in the context of ecological networks [105, 106]. Lafferty et al. [105] reviewed various mechanisms by which parasites can destabilize or stabilize food webs, notably destabilization via the inversion of body size structure [282] and lengthening of trophic chains [154], and stabilization via shared pathogens. All these mechanisms are represented in my model. My finding that parasitism tends to reduce ecosystem variability appears to contradict part of the aforementioned literature, but there is in fact no contradiction because both stabilizing and destabilizing effects occur in my study depending on each of many model realizations with contingent network topologies and dynamics. In addition, the difference of the average result of parasitism from that of Lin & Sutherland [1] could be partly due to the smaller number of species used there in nonparasitic configurations, implying that additional diversity may also have stabilizing effects on average, corroborating current consensus on the biodiversity-stability relationship. Overall, the finding that environmental stochasticity has a far greater role in ecosystem variability than non-predator-prey interactions is not sur- prising insofar as stochasticity is the source of the variability. Nevertheless, the parameter values used for both environmental noise specification and network topology are reasonably realistic in ecological terms, and show that the effects of noise colour are qualitatively similar across different 45 Colour & synchrony of environmental noise combinations of interaction types. Although one could possibly increase the effect sizes of non-predator-prey interactions relative to those of environmental noise by making the former interactions stronger and/or more abundant, and/or making the environmental noise weaker, doing so may compromise realism. These results illustrate the importance of incor- porating stochasticity in ecosystem modeling and prediction, perhaps even more important but even less explored than incorporating non-predator- prey interactions in food webs. This study also shows that for either general understanding or prediction of the dynamics of a specific ecosys- tem, it is important to model the particular kind of stochasticity present in that ecosystem. For example, noise in terrestrial ecosystems tends to be whiter than that in marine ecosystems, and similarly for temperate versus non-temperate latitudes [219]. Overall, the patterns elucidated in this study emerged over and above the considerable variation in network model realizations emulating real-world contingencies across ecosystems. The implications of this study extend to practical conservation. Fo- cusing on single charismatic species without also paying attention to their ecological interactions, or forgetting about cryptic species with dis- proportionate influence, may be an obvious but not necessarily optimal approach [116]. A safer way might be to conserve ecosystem structure and function by protecting a suite of species that represent the widest possible range of ecological functional groups [283] and species that serve a given function but have the widest possible range of responses to different types of stress [284], including responses to environmental noise. Diversity and asynchrony in species responses to noise means less extreme overall fluctuations in ecosystem properties and function. Much research remains to be done to understand how the interactions of a species make it a better or worse candidate for these ends [285]. The results so far support the notion that more systems-oriented research strategies that accommodate stochasticity have an important role to play in cultivating such understanding and helping us make more judicious ecological decisions. 46 Yangchen Lin 3.5 appendix 3.5.1 effects of non-predator-prey interactions Regression trees of effects of non-predator-prey interactions on cv of system energy and exp[Shannon entropy], by noise colours and degrees of synchrony. Explanatory variables: C, direct basal competition; M, mutu- alism; P, parasitism. The categorical explanatory variable levels partitioned to the left-hand branches are indicated (0 and 1 respectively denote absence and presence of the interaction type). Numbers under nodes are mean values of the response variables apportioned to the respective branches above. 47 Colour & synchrony of environm ental noise M = {0} P = {0} C = {0} C = {0} C = {0} P = {0} P = {0} 0:04035 0:03653 0:02731 0:02379 0:03090 0:04570 0:05105 0:04041 0:04417 0:03582 0:04436 0:02736 0:05257 0:05101 0:05412 white no sync C = {0} M = {0} P = {0} P = {0} P = {0} M = {0} M = {0} 0:04305 0:04865 0:04735 0:02983 0:06470 0:04995 0:06560 0:03446 0:03749 0:04360 0:03373 0:05327 0:03141 0:03113 0:03170 white half sync C = {0} M = {0} P = {0} P = {0} M = {0} P = {0} P = {0} 0:04947 0:04759 0:04967 0:03993 0:05930 0:04551 0:05604 0:03508 0:05135 0:04677 0:03996 0:05364 0:05596 0:05908 0:05290 white full sync M = {0} P = {0} C = {0} C = {0} P = {0} C = {0} C = {0} 0:03708 0:03371 0:02775 0:02738 0:02811 0:03964 0:03190 0:04738 0:04048 0:04680 0:04346 0:05011 0:03420 0:03595 0:03248 pink no sync M = {0} P = {0} C = {0} C = {0} P = {0} C = {0} C = {0} 0:2752 0:2961 0:3089 0:3126 0:3051 0:2834 0:2819 0:2849 0:2542 0:2435 0:2491 0:2379 0:2650 0:2720 0:2581 pink half sync M = {0} P = {0} C = {0} C = {0} P = {0} C = {0} C = {0} 0:3716 0:4065 0:4189 0:4191 0:4187 0:3940 0:4031 0:3848 0:3369 0:3098 0:3068 0:3128 0:3640 0:3652 0:3627 pink full sync P = {0} M = {0} C = {0} C = {0} M = {0} C = {0} C = {0} 0:03672 0:03821 0:02860 0:01690 0:04029 0:04782 0:04890 0:04671 0:03524 0:04291 0:04683 0:03899 0:02768 0:03107 0:02433 red no sync M = {0} P = {0} C = {0} C = {0} P = {0} C = {0} C = {0} 0:4595 0:5031 0:5308 0:5380 0:5235 0:4752 0:4839 0:4667 0:4159 0:3958 0:3869 0:4046 0:4358 0:4437 0:4278 red half sync M = {0} P = {0} C = {0} C = {0} P = {0} C = {0} C = {0} 0:6411 0:7037 0:7646 0:7606 0:7686 0:6434 0:6641 0:6226 0:5789 0:5550 0:5702 0:5399 0:6028 0:6116 0:5940 red full sync System energy. 48 Y angchen L in M = {0} P = {0} C = {0} C = {0} P = {0} C = {0} C = {0} 0:03221 0:02986 0:02686 0:02869 0:02500 0:03284 0:03668 0:02904 0:03456 0:04010 0:03484 0:04536 0:02905 0:02380 0:03436 white no sync M = {0} P = {0} C = {0} C = {0} P = {0} C = {0} C = {0} 0:03454 0:03065 0:02858 0:02806 0:02909 0:03269 0:03610 0:02931 0:03842 0:04609 0:05302 0:03929 0:03076 0:02682 0:03470 white half sync C = {0} P = {0} M = {0} M = {0} P = {0} M = {0} M = {0} 0:03857 0:03691 0:03917 0:03522 0:04312 0:03467 0:04095 0:02838 0:04024 0:03923 0:03305 0:04553 0:04126 0:04897 0:03362 white full sync M = {0} P = {0} C = {0} C = {0} C = {0} P = {0} P = {0} 0:03245 0:02768 0:02246 0:02262 0:02230 0:03288 0:02831 0:03745 0:03726 0:03386 0:03674 0:03097 0:04062 0:04450 0:03678 pink no sync P = {0} M = {0} C = {0} C = {0} C = {0} M = {0} M = {0} 0:1182 0:1204 0:1180 0:1167 0:1192 0:1228 0:1197 0:1260 0:1161 0:1175 0:1191 0:1159 0:1146 0:1143 0:1148 pink half sync M = {0} P = {0} C = {0} C = {0} C = {0} P = {0} P = {0} 0:1616 0:1665 0:1595 0:1481 0:1709 0:1735 0:1786 0:1684 0:1567 0:1536 0:1482 0:1589 0:1598 0:1655 0:1541 pink full sync M = {0} P = {0} C = {0} C = {0} P = {0} C = {0} C = {0} 0:03092 0:02807 0:02450 0:01631 0:03269 0:03167 0:03380 0:02954 0:03375 0:04035 0:04250 0:03815 0:02718 0:02721 0:02714 red no sync M = {0} P = {0} C = {0} C = {0} P = {0} C = {0} C = {0} 0:2040 0:2195 0:2419 0:2273 0:2565 0:1969 0:1994 0:1944 0:1884 0:1958 0:1886 0:2030 0:1811 0:1838 0:1783 red half sync P = {0} M = {0} C = {0} C = {0} M = {0} C = {0} C = {0} 0:3115 0:3386 0:3673 0:3781 0:3566 0:3102 0:3050 0:3154 0:2844 0:3021 0:3111 0:2931 0:2667 0:2816 0:2519 red full sync Exponent of Shannon entropy. 49 Colour & synchrony of environmental noise 3.5.2 effects of noise colour and synchrony The influence of noise colour and degree of interspecific synchrony on variability of ecosystem energy and Shannon entropy, in terms of kernel density distributions of outputs from multiple simulations (n ≈ 900), for model configurations other than fcmp (shown in Fig. 3.3 in the main text). a, unsynchronized species environments; b, half synchronized; c, fully synchronized. Violins scaled to constant width. 0.0 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5 Configuration F a 0.0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 1.2 1.4 System energy exp[Shannon entropy] b 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5 0 1 2 Noise colour (spectral exponent) c C V o f ec os y st em m et ri c ov er t im e 50 Yangchen Lin 0.0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 Configuration FC a 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5 System energy exp[Shannon entropy] b 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5 2.0 0 1 2 Noise colour (spectral exponent) c C V o f ec os y st em m et ri c ov er t im e 0.00 0.05 0.10 0.15 0.20 0.25 0.30 Configuration FM a 0.0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 System energy exp[Shannon entropy] b 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5 0 1 2 Noise colour (spectral exponent) c C V o f ec os y st em m et ri c ov er t im e 51 Colour & synchrony of environmental noise 0.00 0.05 0.10 0.15 0.20 0.25 0.30 Configuration FCM a 0.0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 1.2 System energy exp[Shannon entropy] b 0.0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 1.2 1.4 0 1 2 Noise colour (spectral exponent) c C V o f ec os y st em m et ri c ov er t im e 0.0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 1.2 1.4 Configuration FP a 0.0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 1.2 System energy exp[Shannon entropy] b 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5 0 1 2 Noise colour (spectral exponent) c C V o f ec os y st em m et ri c ov er t im e 52 Yangchen Lin 0.0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 Configuration FCP a 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5 System energy exp[Shannon entropy] b 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5 0 1 2 Noise colour (spectral exponent) c C V o f ec os y st em m et ri c ov er t im e 0.0 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5 0.6 Configuration FMP a 0.0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 1.2 System energy exp[Shannon entropy] b 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5 2.0 0 1 2 Noise colour (spectral exponent) c C V o f ec os y st em m et ri c ov er t im e 53 4 extinction and invasion do not add up Abstract. Species extinction and invasion concurrently affect the composition and properties of ecological communities, yet their effects have largely been studied separately, and with more focus on species and ecological functional groups than on the whole-community level. I adopted a dynamic ecological network approach to compare the effects of simultaneous single- species primary extinction and invasion to the effects of extinction and invasion in isolation, using a set of ecosystem metrics. I also investigated the relationship between the impact and reversibility of extinction or invasion through reintroduction or eradication respectively. I used Monte Carlo simulations of bioenergetic ecological network models that combined trophic and mutualistic interactions, contained either prey-dependent or ratio-dependent trophic functional responses, and incorporated either white or pink environmental stochasticity. As the separate extinction or invasion impact increased, the simultaneous extinction-invasion impact increased but was decreasingly additive of the two separate impacts, across all ecosystem metrics. Greater extinction or invasion impact was associated with lower reversibility for most model types and ecosystem metrics. There were also systematic differences between models with prey- and ratio- dependent functional responses. These results highlight the importance of considering the combined effects of extinction and invasion in ecological studies, management and restoration. 4.1 introduction Biodiversity loss is causing changes in ecosystem structure and functioning on a global scale [286], while biotic invasion has been listed as one of the most important global change drivers that influence biotic interactions [287]. Accordingly, there has been extensive research on the impacts of extinction and invasion on communities, with the ecological network approach being increasingly used for understanding and predicting impacts on complex ecosystems [18]. Furthermore, there is increased recognition of 55 Extinction & invasion do not add up the importance of incorporating community dynamics in network studies of extinction and invasion. For example, Curtsdotter et al. [288] found that the effects of sequential species extinctions differ between static and dynamic food webs. Numerous studies have dealt with various aspects of food web structure and dynamics as causes [289–292] and consequences [125, 293, 294] of primary and secondary extinctions at different trophic levels. Because whole-network time-series data are scarce, most such studies are theoretical, although the models are usually constructed and parameterized using empirical network topologies and community dynamics. Nevertheless, they provide community- and ecosystem-level insights that are impractical to obtain empirically. Such studies, however, have been restricted to trophic interactions. Some recent studies have used empirical data to investigate the consequences of extinction on networks of nontrophic interactions either including [116] or excluding [295] trophic interactions, but these studies did not incorporate community dynamics. The effects of invasive species on ecological networks also constitute an area of increasing research (see Olesen et al. [99] p. 44 for a review). Empirical research has progressed from early descriptive studies [296] to recent analyses of long-term data combined with dynamical simulations [297]. Recent theoretical modeling studies have identified predictors of invasion success [126, 298] and which measures best predict invasion impacts on large food webs [30]. In terms of the effects of invasion on nontrophic networks, most research has been on bipartite mutualistic networks of pollinators and seed dispersers [299–301] and, as with extinction studies, largely restricted to network topology without community dynamics [302– 304]. A few studies have examined other kinds of mutualistic [305] or parasitic networks [306] restricted to particular pairs of taxonomic groups. Existing network research on the effects of extinction or invasion has one or more of the following limitations: they do not incorporate community dynamics, the models contain only one interaction type, the models are deterministic, or the metrics of perturbation impact pertain to population- rather than community- or ecosystem-level properties. 56 Yangchen Lin Indeed, responses of ecosystems as a whole to invasions are much less known than responses of populations or communities [307]. Furthermore, the vast majority of studies of the impacts of extinction and invasion on ecological networks have so far examined extinction and invasion in isolation from each other, or at invasion as a cause of extinction [308]. There is a lack of studies examining the combined effects of simultaneous extinction and invasion driven by separate causes [309]. The importance of examining both processes concurrently is suggested by Forys & Allen’s [310] empirical study of functional group change caused by extinctions and invasions, and Jackson & Sax [311] who promulgate the notion of ‘biodiversity dynamics’ in a changing environment as being the shifting balance between species loss and gain. Indeed, non-native species introduced by humans may mitigate the global ‘trophic downgrading’ of food webs [312, 313]. Reintroduction of extinct species and eradication of invasive species are increasingly being carried out in restoration ecology; an understanding of the reversibility of extinction and invasion is therefore crucial to restoration [314]. Lundberg et al. [315] found in dynamic models of competitive communities that cascading extinctions could sometimes cause community changes that preclude reinvasion, although they did not include trophic and other types of nontrophic interactions and treated reinvasion as a binary rather than continuous variable. The eradication of invasive species can also have unexpected ecosystem-level outcomes because of species interactions (review by Zavaleta et al. [316]). For example, alien plant removal has been found to have negative impacts on rare native plants in pollinator networks [317]. The importance of adopting a network perspective on ecological restoration is increasingly being recognized [116, 300]. Existing studies are mostly empirical investigations of specific taxa and habitats (see Kardol & Wardle [314] and references therein); more recent research has begun to demonstrate the role of network simulation models. For example, Raymond et al. [318] dynamically simulated the effects of invasive species eradication in a subantarctic island’s species interaction network using and comparing different model 57 Extinction & invasion do not add up structures, although they used random interaction strengths and did not metabolically scale parameter values. As Wardle et al. [309] state about the effects of alien consumers, ‘we have yet to move from a collection of impressive examples to the development of general principles’. In my study, ‘general principles’ are sought for the impacts and reversibilities of extinction and invasion in ecological networks, where extinctions and invasions are driven by independent processes. I also attempt to address issues recently identified as high ecological research priorities [200, 307]: the role of rare species in ecosystem functioning, what kinds of invasive species will affect ecosystem properties, and how changes in ecosystem properties are related to changes in community structure. My strategy entails ensemble simulations to seek universal patterns in the impacts of extinction and invasion (if any) across multiple contexts. Empirical studies have limitations such as being snapshots in time, which do not capture the actual mechanisms of biodiversity loss, or short-term studies capturing mostly transient effects rather than long-term impacts [319]. There can also be biogeographical or taxonomical biases, as has been seen in field studies of the impacts of invasive alien plants [320]. This is where simulation modeling can provide valuable insights. More specifically, I look at the effects of network connectance and various extinctor (species going extinct) and invader species traits on the impacts of primary extinction and invasion, the impact of simultaneous extinction and invasion, and the reversibility of extinction and invasion via reintroduction and eradication respectively. The impacts and reversibil- ities are quantified in terms of changes in several ecosystem metrics representing community structure, biomass, diversity and periodicity. This multidimensional approach to quantifying ecological stability gives a more comprehensive understanding, anticipation and management of effects of perturbation on ecosystems [321]. In addition, it may be important to ac- count for real-world environmental fluctuations in models; for example, Joo et al. [322] found that deterministic models of bacteriophage-mediated bac- terial invasion did not adequately capture dynamics that involved stochastic 58 Yangchen Lin processes. This is the first study to incorporate positive interactions and environmental stochasticity into bioenergetic food web modeling to inves- tigate how extinction, invasion and combined impacts propagate through noisy dynamics of species interactions to influence properties at the ecosys- tem level of organization. This may ultimately help determine extinction and invasion management actions with the best chance of success [323]. 4.2 methods The stochastic model is based on that documented in Chapter 2. Models of 11-species communities were generated with all different combinations of niche and random trophic network topologies, prey- and ratio-dependent functional responses and white and pink noise. No parasitism or direct basal competition was implemented, for comparability across niche and random network topologies. Trophic and mutualistic connectances were varied within the range [0:05; 0:3] (similar to Romanuk et al. [126]) using a Sobol’ low-discrepancy sequence [324] across replicates within each topology-dynamics-noise configuration. The degree of interspecific syn- chrony of response to noise was set at half-synchyronized for all models (see Chapter 3). Dynamical parameter values are listed in Table 3.1. The starting sample size for each topology-dynamics-noise configura- tion was 100. Within each of the 100 replicates, four ‘parallel universe’ simulations were set up with the same parameters, initial conditions and pseudorandom seed states, but with different combinations of extinction and invasion (Fig. 4.1). For each simulation within a replicate, one ran- domly selected species was removed prior to the start of the simulations, to subsequently invade the system in the relevant simulations. Similarly, another species was randomly selected for future extinction. For each extinctor and invader, several species traits were quanti- fied. These, and the trophic and mutualistic connectances, constitute the explanatory variables (Table 4.1) whose effects on the impacts and re- versibilities of extinction and invasion were investigated using regression trees using the tree package in R [275]. The effects of individual ex- planatory variables on the absolute magnitudes of the response variables 59 Extinction & invasion do not add up (except extinction-invasion interaction) were also analyzed separately using Kendall correlations, or Mann-Whitney j-tests in the case of categorical variables (extinctor and invader basality). This procedure was then repeated with [extinction and/or invasion impacts on community structure] as an explanatory variable, against extinction and/or invasion impacts on the five other ecosystem metrics. Time (arbitrary units) a b c d e a d b e c control invasion eradication extinction reintroduction both 0 2000 4000 6000 8000 10000 12000 Figure 4.1. Simulation experiment design. Each of the four rows symbolizes a single stochastic simulation of multispecies dynamics. The four simulations had identical starting model structure, invader and/or extinctor species, parameter values, initial conditions and noise time series. The diagram as a whole represents one of 100 replicates for each noise colour within each type of dynamics (prey-dependent and ratio-dependent) within each network topology (niche model and random). The red segments denote the parts of the time series over which various ecosystem metrics were calculated. The difference in each metric between pairs of time series was calculated as indicated by a (impact of invasion), b (impact of extinction), c (impact of simultaneous invasion and extinction), d (reversibility of invasion, by invader eradication) and e (reversibility of extinction, by extinctor reintroduction). 60 Y angchen L in Table 4.1. Abbreviations and description of explanatory variables. Abbreviation Description trocon trophic connectance mutcon mutualistic connectance invbasalnon is invader a basal species extbasalnon is extinctor a basal species invniche invader niche value (trophic level) extniche extinctor niche value (trophic level) invsim invader mean interaction similarity (modified from Romanuk et al. [126] p. 1746 to combine trophic and nontrophic interactions) extsim extinctor mean interaction similarity (modified from Romanuk et al. [126]) iesim mean interaction similarity of invader and extinctor (modified from Romanuk et al. [126]) ipred no¯ of consumer species of invader iprey no¯ of resource species of invader imut no¯ of mutualiser species with invader epred no¯ of consumer species of extinctor eprey no¯ of resource species of extinctor emut no¯ of mutualiser species with extinctor dominance relative extinctor abundance prior to extinction, when not under extinction pressure (mean species energy over 2( (() ≤ time t ≤ 4( ((( divided by mean total system energy over same period) ilink total no¯ of network links of invader elink total no¯ of network links of extinctor 61 Extinction & invasion do not add up Extinction, invasion or simultaneous extinction and invasion was en- forced at time t = 40 001, and reintroduction or eradication at t = 80 001. A species was made extinct by instantaneously setting its state to zero. This implementation is the most straightforward in representing different causes of mortality. Isolated network clusters may occasionally be created by species removal, but this does not violate ecological laws and the separate clusters are still resource-limited. If a cluster ended up with no basal species, it would die out as expected in reality. The state of an invader at the time of invasion was set at 10% of what its initial state would be if it were present as a native species at the start of the simulation, as invasions usually begin with a few individuals; simi- larly, species previously made extinct were reintroduced at 10% of their initial states. Invader eradication was similar to extinction, with species state instantaneously set to zero. Six ecosystem metrics (Table 4.2) were calculated from time steps 20 001 to 40 000 (before extinction or inva- sion), 60 001 to 80 000 (after extinction or invasion) and 100 001 to 120 000 (after reintroduction or eradication) to allow for most transients to pass and to avoid capturing any spurious rate-dependent hysteresis caused by rapid extinction. Preliminary simulations indicated that this time scale of measurement encompassed all the main features and periodicities of the stochastic behaviour. 62 Y angchen L in Table 4.2. Ecosystem metrics. Metric Description cv energy coefficient of variation over time of total biomass of all species cv entropy coefficient of variation over time of ]pp[Shannon entropy] of relative species biomasses [273] average energy average over time of total biomass of all species average entropy average over time of ]pp[Shannon entropy] of relative species biomasses [273] community structure set of all species states at each time step power spectrum amplitude distribution of frequencies of biomass fluctuation [225] of each species 63 Extinction & invasion do not add up Impacts of extinction, invasion and simultaneous extinction and invasion (hereafter referred to as ‘extinction-invasion impact’) were quantified by comparing the ecosystem metrics of the perturbed versus control simulations. Differences between parallel simulations make better measures of extinction and invasion impacts than measuring the same simulation before and after perturbation, because in the latter we would not know whether any changes in the system after extinction or invasion were actually caused by the perturbation or not. Differences in the means and coefficients of variation of energy and entropy were calculated as proportionate change, with the control simulation as the baseline. The difference in community structure was quantified as the absolute root mean square deviation of the time series of a given species between the impacted system and the same time steps in the control simulation (i.e. as if nothing happened throughout), calculated species by species, and then summed across all species and divided by the number of time steps, given byvuuut S∑s=1∑t {(Es;tpost;start − Es;tpre;start)2; : : : ; (Es;tpost;end − Es;tpre;end)2} tpre;end − tpre;start + 1 (4.1) where the subscripts pre and post indicate pre- and post-decline periods, and start and end refer to the first and last time steps at which the ecosystem metrics were measured. The difference in the log power spectrum was quantified as for community structure. The power spectrum, calculated as one of the ecosystem metrics using default settings in the spectrum function in R, is informative about the ‘internal dynamics’ of the system [214, 225, 325]. By revealing the detailed relationship between power and frequency, this measure paints a picture of stability and periodicity at different time scales—the system can be stable at one scale and unstable at another [326]. It can be indicative of changes in the interaction between stochasticity and deterministic density-dependent cycles. The power spectrum was calculated over 10 000 uniform frequency intervals from 0:000 05 to 0:5 (the Nyquist 64 Yangchen Lin frequency) cycles per time step. An alternative way to measure change in power spectra is by means of spectral entropy, or Shannon entropy of the power spectrum [327], but this method loses frequency information—a spectrum that is a mirror image of another will have identical entropy. In addition to measuring the extinction-invasion impact per se, I also developed a measure of the interaction between extinction and invasion, hereafter referred to as the extinction-invasion interaction. For each ecosystem metric, the extinction-invasion interaction was calculated as (extinction-invasion impact)− [(extinction impact) + (invasion impact)] (4.2) from the parallel simulations. The expected impact of simultaneous extinc- tion and invasion if they were purely additive is [(impact of extinction)+ (impact of invasion)]. If the interaction has a value of 0, extinction and invasion do not interact (i.e. have purely additive effects). In the case of changes in community structure and power spectra, which are always positive, a negative interaction value means that extinction and invasion mitigate each other’s impact while a positive value means that they re- inforce each other. In the case of changes in average or coefficient of variation of energy and entropy, which can take positive or negative values depending on whether the extinction and/or invasion causes them to increase or decrease, a negative interaction value simply means that the interaction between co-occurring extinction and invasion is less than purely additive, while a positive value indicates the opposite. In the case of coefficient of variation of energy or entropy, positive may be detrimental because it indicates that the variablility has increased i.e. stability is lower; in the case of average energy or entropy, positive may be beneficial as it indicates that the overall biomass or diversity has increased. Reversibility of extinction or invasion depends on two factors: the magnitude of the impact of extinction or invasion, and the extent to which the system can return to its pre-extinction or pre-invasion characteristics following reintroduction or eradication respectively. The impact was quan- 65 Extinction & invasion do not add up tified as above, while the difference of the post-reintroduction/eradication ecosystem from the control simulation where no disturbance ever happened (this difference henceforth referred to as ‘reintroduction or eradication effect’) was quantified by calculating the differences in the above metrics after reintroduction/eradication compared to the same time steps in the control simulation. The ‘relative reversibility’ of extinction or invasion— relative to the extinction or invasion impact—was then calculated as extinction or invasion impact(reintroduction or eradication effect) + 10−100 . (4.3) Thus the perturbation is relatively more reversible if after a larger perturbation the system reverts to its unaffected condition. The addition of 10−100 avoided division by 0; the value was chosen to be several orders of magnitude smaller than the smallest non-zero data point. As reversibility is a non-negative concept, what matters here is the magnitude but not the sign of the impacts, so the absolute value is taken. To see how the reintroduction or eradication effect itself is related to the impact of extinction or invasion, ‘absolute reversibility’ was calculated as 1 minus the sample-normalized absolute values of the reintroduction or eradication effect, where the smaller the effect, the greater the absolute reversibility: 1− |reintroduction or eradication effect| max|reintroduction or eradication effect| . (4.4) Finally, the overall distributions of the response variables were com- pared across topology-dynamics-noise configurations, to probe the relative extents to which model type matters to model outputs. For the energy- and entropy-related response variables, which could take on either posi- tive or negative values, the distributions would also indicate the relative directionality of the effects in addition to their magnitudes as described above. 66 Yangchen Lin 4.3 results Replicates were discarded in which noise terms and numerical integration errors resulted in negative species states. The final sample size of each topology-dynamics-noise configuration ranged from 72 to 98. Relatively extreme values of response variables occurred in a small proportion of replicates because some of the response variables, being proportionate changes in some quantity, are divided by the original value of the quantity which sometimes takes on very small fractional values, when populations are very small. This is ecologically plausible and not abnormal, especially as species states are in arbitrary units. The regression trees showed no consistent interaction structure among the explanatory variables of web connectances and species traits. The explanatory variable explaining most of the deviance for each topology- dynamics-noise configuration is listed in Appendix 4.5.1. For extinction impact and extinction-invasion impact in most topology-dynamics-noise configurations, extinctor dominance often accounts for most of the de- viance in changes in power spectra and average entropy. Invader niche value usually explains most of the deviance of invasion reversibility in ratio-dependent niche models, across noise colours and ecosystem metrics. An explanatory variable that explains most of the deviance in a regression tree is not necessarily highly correlated with the response variable. Tables of effects of individual explanatory variables on magnitudes of response variables are found in §3.5 of the online supplement to Lin & Sutherland [2] at http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.baae.2014.07.008. Extinctor dominance is moderately strongly correlated to extinction impact for many model types and ecosystem metrics across both white and pink noise, in addition to explaining most of the deviance in many of the regression trees. The correlations are mostly positive for all ecosystem metrics except power spectrum, where the correlations are all negative. The number of invader predators is usually moderately to moderately strongly negatively correlated to invasion reversibility for all model types except prey-dependent niche models, despite not often explaining most 67 Extinction & invasion do not add up of the deviance in regression trees. Relationships for other explanatory and response variables are mostly insignificant in comparison. Relationships between impacts on community structure and impacts on the other five community metrics are shown in Fig. 4.2 (tabulated in Appendix 4.5.2). There are mostly moderately strong positive correlations between community structure and energy- and entropy-related response variables, across model types and noise colours (sample sizes: prey- dependent models, 387; ratio-dependent models, 319). Correlations of community structure with power spectra are generally not as strong, but there is a notable difference between the positive correlations for prey- dependent models and negative correlations for ratio-dependent models in terms of invasion impact. 68 Y angchen L in CV energy 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5 E x t i n c t i o n CV entropy 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5 Average energy 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5 Average entropy 0.0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 1.2 1.4 Power spectrum 1.48 1.50 1.52 1.54 1.56 a r c t a n a b s o l u t e i m p a c t o n m e t r i c 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5 I n v a s i o n 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5 0.0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 1.2 1.4 1.44 1.46 1.48 1.50 1.52 1.54 1.56 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5 I n v a s . + E x t . 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5 arctan impact on community structure 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5 0.0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 1.2 1.4 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5 1.51 1.52 1.53 1.54 1.55 1.56 1.57 Figure 4.2. Relationships between impacts on community structure and absolute values of impacts on the other ecosystem metrics, pooled across network topologies and noise colours within each of prey-dependent (black triangles) and ratio- dependent (red squares) model types. Apparent bimodality of data points is an artifact of arctangent transformation which facilitates visual comparisons but compresses large values into a small range. Appendix 4.5.2 gives correlations of untransformed data. 69 E xtinction & invasion do not add up ● ● ●●●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ●● 0.5 1.0 1.5 0.5 1.0 1.5 0.5 1.0 1.5 E I ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●●●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●●● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●●●● ●● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● 1.45 1.50 1.55 1.48 1.50 1.52 1.54 1.561.52 1.54 1.56 1.58 E I ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●●● ●● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● −0.5 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5 −0.5 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5 −1 0 1 2 E I ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● −0.5 0.0 0.5 1.0 −0.5 0.0 0.5 1.0 −1 0 1 2 E I ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ●●● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● −0.5 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5 −0.5 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5 −1 0 1 E I ●●●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● −0.5 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5 −0.5 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5−0.5 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5 E I a Community structure Average energy CV energy Power spectrum Average entropy CV entropy E x t i n c t i o n - i n v a s i o n i m p a c t ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ●● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ●●● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● 0.5 1.0 1.5 0.5 1.0 1.5−1 0 1 E I ● ●●●● ●●●● ●●● ●●●●● ●●● ●●● ●●● ●● ● ●●● ● ● ●●●●●● ●●●●● ●●● ●●● ●●● ●●● ●●●●● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ●●● ●● ● ● ●● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●●● ●●●●● ●● ●●● ●●●●● ●● ●●●● ● ● ●● ●●● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ●●● ●● ●●● ●● ●●● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ●●●● ●● ●●●● ●●● ●● ● ●● ●●● ● ●●● ●● ● ●●● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ●●●● ● ●● ● ● ●●●● ● ●● ●● ●● ●●●● ●● ●● ● ● ●●●●● ●● ● ●● ●● ●●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● 1.45 1.50 1.55 1.48 1.50 1.52 1.54 1.56 −1 0 1 E I ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●●●● ● ●● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ●●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●●●● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ●● ●●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●●●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● −0.5 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5 −0.5 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5−1 0 1 E I ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●●●● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●●●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ●●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● −0.5 0.0 0.5 1.0 −0.5 0.0 0.5 1.0 −1.0 −0.5 0.0 0.5 1.0 E I ● ●● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● −0.5 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5 −0.5 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5−1 0 1 E I ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● −0.5 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5 −0.5 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5−1 0 1 E I b Community structure Average energy CV energy Power spectrum Average entropy CV entropy E x t i n c t i o n - i n v a s i o n i n t e r a c t i o n Figure 4.3. Relationships of separate extinction and invasion impacts to (a) extinction-invasion impact and (b) extinction- invasion interaction, pooled across model types and noise colours (n 5 7(6), with fitted generalized additive model surface. Axes: E, extinction impact; I, invasion impact; all arctangent transformed for visual interpretability. Data points coloured according to relative extinction-invasion impact, for clarity. Occasional bimodality of data points is an artifact of arctangent transformation that does not affect the statistical analysis. In b, impacts of extinction and invasion are purely additive when extinction-invasion interaction 5 (. 70 Y angchen L in Table 4.3. Kendall correlations of separate impacts of extinction and invasion to extinction- invasion impact and to extinction-invasion interaction, pooled across model types and noise colours. Extinction-invasion… Ecosystem metric ext pext inv pinv impact community structure (:62) ( (:6(5 ( cv energy (:404 ( (:301 ( cv entropy (:42 ( (:300 ( average energy (:5(1 ( (:43) ( average entropy (:5)5 ( (:344 ( power spectrum (:547 ( (:550 ( interaction community structure −(:400 2:75× )(−84 −(:552 3:27× )(−107 cv energy −(:257 ):70× )(−24 −(:)7 ):)4× )(−11 cv entropy −(:235 0:5)× )(−21 −(:202 2:42× )(−29 average energy −(:(7(5 (:((5(5 −(:)23 1:66× )(−7 average entropy −(:(45) (:(726 −(:2(4 5:32× )(−16 power spectrum −(:47 3:13× )(−78 −(:47) ):01× )(−78 Subscripts: ext, extinction; inv, invasion. Sample size n 5 7(6, significance level 5 (:((033 after Bonferroni correction. 71 Extinction & invasion do not add up Kendall correlations were calculated for impact of extinction against extinction-invasion impact and against the extinction-invasion interaction, and for impact of invasion against those same two variables (n = 706). The analysis was done on data pooled across all model types and noise colours, since there was little qualitative difference between them. Increasing impact of extinction or invasion is strongly associated with increasing extinction- invasion impact (Fig. 4.3a, Table 4.3). The extinction-invasion interaction, however, becomes less additive and, as separate positive extinction and invasion impacts increase, smaller than what it would be if the impacts of extinction and invasion were purely additive (Fig. 4.3b, Table 4.3). The relationship between extinction impact and absolute reversibility, and between invasion impact and absolute reversibility, are shown in Fig. 4.4. Niche and random network topologies and white and pink noise colours were pooled within each of prey-dependent and ratio-dependent model types (sample sizes: prey-dependent models, 387; ratio-dependent models, 319), since there were no qualitative differences in the data between the pooled categories. All correlations are negative and often weak to moderately strong. Impacts of extinction and invasion are usually more strongly correlated with their respective reversibilities for prey-dependent models than for ratio-dependent models. Across all topology-dynamics-noise configurations, differences in the outputs of prey- and ratio-dependent models are generally greater than those between different network topologies and noise colours, except for power spectra where niche and random network topologies make the greatest difference (Fig. 4.5; sample sizes: prey-dependent niche, 194; prey-dependent random, 193; ratio-dependent niche, 172; ratio-dependent random, 147). Ratio-dependent models tend to produce less variable impacts, reversibilities and extinction-invasion interactions, except for power spectra. As expected, extinction tends to reduce average energy and entropy while invasion tends to increase them (Fig. 4.5a), and extinction and invasion tend to cancel out each other’s effects (in a statistical-distribution context) when combined (comparing Figs. 4.5a and 4.5b). The directionality of energy- and entropy-related impacts, however, 72 Yangchen Lin is less symmetric, the upper tails of the distributions tending to be longer than the lower tails for both extinction and invasion (upper rows of Figs. 4.5a and 4.5b). Interestingly, the shapes of distributions are more similar between extinction impact and invasion impact for a given model type than they are across model types within either extinction or invasion (Fig. 4.5a). The extinction-invasion interaction is mostly near zero (i.e. almost purely additive impacts of extinction and invasion) for energy and entropy-related ecosystem metrics, but tends to negative and highly negative for community structure and power spectra respectively, but this is because of the abundance of high extinction-invasion impacts for those metrics. 73 E xtinction & invasion do not add up Figure 4.4 (facing page). Relationship between (a) extinction impact and absolute reversibility and (b) invasion impact and absolute reversibility, pooled across network topologies and noise colours within each of prey-dependent (upper row) and ratio-dependent (lower row) model types. Kendall  signifi- cance level 5 (:((033 after Bonferroni correction. Legend: triangles/circles, niche/random network topologies; black/red data points, white/pink noise. 74 Y angchen L in Community struct. ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● 0.0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 E x t i n c t i o n r e v e r s i b i l i t y τ = −0.545 p = 7.93e−58 ● ● ●●● ● ●●●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ●● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● 0.0 0.4 0.8 0.0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 τ = −0.244 p = 1.01e−10 a CV energy ●● ●● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● τ = −0.298 p = 2.36e−18 ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●●● ● ● ●● ●●●● ●● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● 0.0 0.4 0.8 τ = −0.117 p = 0.00183 CV entropy ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● τ = −0.282 p = 1.39e−16 ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● 0.0 0.4 0.8 | normalized extinction impact | τ = −0.0364 p = 0.334 Average energy ●● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ●●● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● τ = −0.342 p = 1.14e−23 ● ●● ●●●● ●● ●●● ●●● ●● ●●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● 0.0 0.4 0.8 τ = −0.121 p = 0.00134 Average entropy ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●●● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●●● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● τ = −0.331 p = 2.71e−22 ● ●● ●●●●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ●● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● 0.0 0.4 0.8 τ = −0.0953 p = 0.0116 Power spectrum ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●●● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● τ = −0.533 p = 2.32e−55 ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● 0.0 0.4 0.8 τ = −0.468 p = 7.49e−36 Community struct. ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● 0.0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 I n v a s i o n r e v e r s i b i l i t y τ = −0.476 p = 1.88e−44 ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ●● ●●● ●● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● 0.0 0.4 0.8 0.0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 τ = −0.0587 p = 0.117 b CV energy ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● τ = −0.265 p = 5.97e−15 ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ●● ● ●●●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● 0.0 0.4 0.8 τ = −0.183 p = 1.09e−06 CV entropy ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ●●● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● τ = −0.119 p = 0.00049 ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ●●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● 0.0 0.4 0.8 | normalized invasion impact | τ = −0.226 p = 1.54e−09 Average energy ● ●● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● τ = −0.319 p = 6.01e−21 ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● 0.0 0.4 0.8 τ = −0.12 p = 0.00142 Average entropy ● ●● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● τ = −0.178 p = 1.66e−07 ● ● ● ●●● ● ●● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● 0.0 0.4 0.8 τ = −0.0483 p = 0.198 Power spectrum ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● τ = −0.613 p = 1.05e−72 ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ●●● ● 0.0 0.4 0.8 τ = −0.38 p = 2.86e−24 75 E xtinction & invasion do not add up Figure 4.5 (facing page). Effects of model type on (a) the im- pacts (upper row of a) and relative reversibilities (lower row of a) of extinction (dashed lines) and invasion (solid lines) and (b) the extinction-invasion impact (upper row of b) and extinction-invasion interaction (lower row of b). Black half- violins, white noise; red half-violins, pink noise. Model types: PDn, Prey-Dependent niche model; RDr, Ratio-Dependent random-network model. Violins scaled to constant width and connected at medians. The arctangent transformation produces the artifact of bimodality in some violins, but facilitates visual comparisons. Statistical analyses used untransformed data. 76 Y angchen L in 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5 Community structure −0.5 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5 CV energy −0.5 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5 CV entropy −0.5 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5 Average energy −0.5 0.0 0.5 1.0 Average entropy 1.44 1.46 1.48 1.50 1.52 1.54 1.56 Power spectrum a r c t a n i m p a c t a 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5 PDn RDnPDr RDr 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5 PDn RDnPDr RDr 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5 PDn RDnPDr RDr 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5 PDn RDnPDr RDr 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5 PDn RDnPDr RDr 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 1.2 1.4 1.6 PDn RDnPDr RDr a r c t a n r e v e r s i b i l i t y 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5 Community structure −0.5 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5 CV energy −0.5 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5 CV entropy −0.5 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5 Average energy −0.5 0.0 0.5 1.0 Average entropy 1.51 1.52 1.53 1.54 1.55 1.56 1.57 Power spectrum a r c t a n E - I i m p a c t b −1.5 −1.0 −0.5 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5 PDn RDnPDr RDr −1.5 −1.0 −0.5 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5 PDn RDnPDr RDr −1.5 −1.0 −0.5 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5 PDn RDnPDr RDr −1.5 −1.0 −0.5 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5 PDn RDnPDr RDr −1.0 −0.5 0.0 0.5 1.0 PDn RDnPDr RDr −1.5 −1.0 −0.5 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5 PDn RDnPDr RDr a r c t a n E - I i n t e r a c t i o n 77 Extinction & invasion do not add up 4.4 discussion That more dominant species tend to have greater impacts after extinction is not surprising, since one would expect the loss of a dominant species constituting a large proportion of the abundance or biomass to bring a greater change to community structure, system energy and entropy. For extinction-invasion impact, the effect of dominance may have been weakened by the impact of invasion. Interestingly, consistent negative rather than positive correlations of extinctor dominance with changes in power spectra were observed. This may be because dominant extinctors tend to have fewer predators (Appendix 4.5.3a); the extinction of a sparsely linked dominant species may cause less disruption to the overall profile of interacting population and noise cycles. The relationship between extinctor dominance and number of predators was not substantially due to niche model structure and metabolic scaling (Appendix 4.5.3b). Question 55 of Sutherland et al. [200] asks, ‘How important are rare species in the functioning of ecological communities?’ These results suggest that rare species may not necessarily be more important than common (dominant) species in the functioning of ecological communities. Rare species may, however, be more prone to total extinction owing to their rarity, and there may be cumulative impacts if multiple rare species are lost concurrently. Where historical data are lacking, it may also be difficult to distinguish between species that are naturally rare and species that are normally common but are rare at sampling time because they are in the process of going extinct. Strayer [307] suggested that there does not seem to be a general answer to the question of which invasions will change ecosystem function; my results support this view that the impact of invasion is highly context-dependent. Similarly, insights into Question 50 of Sutherland et al. [200]—‘How relevant are assembly rules in a world of biological invasion?’—can be gained from my results in that assembly rules, insofar as the rules relate to the degree of niche overlap of the invasive and native species, may have limited predictive power in invasion given the 78 Yangchen Lin multitude of counteracting factors. For example, for invasion success per se, it is widely recognized that greater niche overlap may make it harder to invade, but a smaller overlap may mean that the invader is less well- adapted to the ecosystem than the natives and therefore less successful in invasion [328]. This tradeoff may carry over to the invasion impact on the ecosystem as a whole, and be further tempered by other factors. My results support Simberloff’s [323] prognosis that invasive species management decisions should be considered on a case-by-case basis, and underscore the importance of multi-pronged strategies synthesizing other lines of inquiry as disparate as invasive species distribution modeling [329] and risk and decision analysis [330, 331]. Community ecology and network dynamics are essential components of any such strategy, as might be illustrated by a case of unexpected community consequences of a species introduced for biological control [332]. For both extinction and invasion (and the two combined), larger changes in community structure can potentially be accompanied by larger changes in energy- and entropy-related ecosystem metrics, as expected, although large changes in community structure are more likely to accom- pany small than large changes in the energy- and entropy-related metrics given the distribution of points in Fig. 4.2. Nevertheless, the results imply that if large changes in the variability or magnitude of biomass or overall species evenness are observed in an ecosystem of interest, it may be generally expected that there has been a considerable change in community structure that could potentially translate to changes in various aspects of ecosystem function and ecosystem services. The extinction, invasion and extinction-invasion impacts on energy- and entropy-related ecosystem metrics are similarly likely to be negative or positive in most cases (Fig. 4.5), suggesting that extinction and invasion can be both beneficial and detrimental in terms of ecosystem function, insofar as high carbon storage (system energy) and high diversity (Shannon entropy) are widely recognized to be of ecological benefit. This outcome supports shifting attitudes towards invasive species partly brought about by a more system-oriented world view, with increasing debate on both 79 Extinction & invasion do not add up positive and negative impacts at the ecosystem and socioeconomic levels [333–335]. The upper tails of the distributions of those impacts tend to be longer than the lower tails; this is attributed not to any ecological phenomenon but to the measurement of impacts as proportionate change, which sometimes generates large positive values as explained in §4.3. In most of the models, the magnitude of the impacts on energy- and entropy-related metrics is relatively small, partly due to just a single extinctor or invader. The impacts on community structure and power spectra, however, take on a wider range of magnitudes, probably because these metrics deal with more mechanistic aspects of the ecosystem’s behaviour, which can be affected in many qualitatively different ways, rather than the total energy and diversity. In cases where separate impacts of extinction and invasion are high, the extinction-invasion impact increases but the extinction-invasion interaction becomes increasingly less than purely additive. This might be attributable to a kind of system- level ‘density dependence’ arising from the intrinsic resistance of the system to perturbation, due to reproduction- and mortality-mediated negative feedback loops acting within and across interacting species. Such homeostasis, however, may break down with multiple invasions and extinctions including secondary extinctions, and should not therefore be taken for granted in the maintenance of ecosystem function. The difficulty of reversing extinctions or invasions with large impacts may be discouraging, but is not unexpected news. In the case of extinction, this may be due to secondary extinction cascades that cannot be reversed by simply reintroducing one species. In the case of invasion, it may be due to invasive species having self-reinforcing effects on ecosystem processes over time [336]. The results suggest that reversal efforts may sometimes not be worth the difficulty, because of either small extinction or invasion impact or low reversibility. One, however, needs to consider reintroduction or eradication on a case-by-case basis, as the desired end result need not be identical to the original state, and there have been spectacular success stories in wolf reintroduction [337] and rat eradication [338]. Further research in simultaneous extinction and invasion and their 80 Yangchen Lin reversibilities will also help in reconciling the sometimes conflicting goals of endangered species management and invasive species eradication [339]. It has also been reported that complex interactions among multiple invading species can affect invasion impact and reversibility on native species [340]; this kind of approach could be extended to account for network structure and dynamics. Differences in behaviour between prey- and ratio-dependent models tend to be more pronounced than differences in topology or even whether extinction or invasion is involved. If niche models are considered as much more appropriate than random networks, then perhaps more attention should correspondingly be paid to whether prey or ratio dependence is more ecologically appropriate. The proportionate fluctuations of predators and prey that characterize ratio dependence may be responsible for the differences from prey-dependent models observed in this study, such as smaller variability in model behaviour across models of the same type of dependence. This recalls the long-debated question as to whether prey- or ratio-dependent models better represent biomass distributions in the real world, and underscores the need to ‘get it right’ in predictive models of specific systems. The median extinction, invasion and extinction-invasion impacts on power spectra are considerably higher for models with random topology than those with niche topology (Fig. 4.5). This is possibly because random networks are less hierarchical and less nested than niche-structured networks; when one perturbs a niche model, it is more likely that there are surviving species more similar to the extinctor and/or invader to maintain the periodicity structure than when one perturbs a random model. Random networks also translate to more trophic complementarity (sensu Poisot et al. [341]) in the network overall; this implies that trophic complementarity is potentially a predictor of the structure of community periodicity, in addition to the other ecosystem functions studied by Poisot et al. [341]. Nevertheless, looking at the rest of the ecosystem metrics other than power spectra, it appears that communities with interaction topologies that do not conform to the traditional niche model will not 81 Extinction & invasion do not add up behave in radically different ways from those that obey niche rules; indeed, Solé & Valverde [15] regarded food webs in general as having relatively high randomness. At the same time as general relationships were found across contingen- cies of model structure and parameter values,7 the considerable variation in model behaviour reflects real-world ecological variation and context dependence of extinction and invasion impacts [307, 342], potentially across both aquatic and terrestrial ecosystems. Future work along the lines of this study will provide further insights into how we might influence ecological network dynamics through selection of species (nodes) to eradi- cate or reintroduce for increased recovery and resilience sensu Cornelius et al. [343], perhaps using techniques inspired by control theory [344] and appreciating that overall network topology may be more important than individual node degree as a determinant of resilience [345]. Also worthy of further inquiry is the relatively little known phenomenon of degeneracy [77], in which species differing in traits can perform similar functions, potentially mediating the impacts of extinction or invasion in unexpected ways. Finally, it may also be mutually beneficial for different disciplines to understand the similarities and differences between ecological and other kinds of networks in their responses to perturbations [346] and to control measures [347–350]. At the same time, it is important to bear in mind that the time-irreversibility of complex dynamical processes [8] precludes perfect reversal of disturbances. As ecology matures into a predictive discipline alongside the other sciences, the systems approach offers rich prospects for exciting new fundamental research and more effective environmental management. 7The similar outcome across noise colours does not contradict Chapter 3, because the concern here is with the impacts of invasion and extinction and not with the ‘day-to-day’ behaviour of the system per se. Modeling noise correctly for any given system is still important if more specific predictions are desired. 82 Yangchen Lin 4.5 appendix 4.5.1 regression trees Root predictors that explain the largest proportions of deviances in extinction and invasion impacts, extinction-invasion interaction and relative reversibilities, for different model types, noise colours and ecosystem metrics. See Table 4.1 in main text for key to predictor abbreviations. 83 Extinction & invasion do not add up Predictors of extinction impact. Ecosystem metric Model type Noise Root predictor community structure prey-dependent niche white dominance pink extsim prey-dependent random white extbasalnon pink dominance ratio-dependent niche white epred pink extsim ratio-dependent random white dominance pink dominance cv energy prey-dependent niche white eprey pink dominance prey-dependent random white elink pink extniche ratio-dependent niche white dominance pink extbasalnon ratio-dependent random white elink pink dominance cv entropy prey-dependent niche white eprey pink eprey prey-dependent random white extniche pink extniche ratio-dependent niche white extsim pink emut ratio-dependent random white dominance pink extniche average energy prey-dependent niche white eprey pink extniche prey-dependent random white elink pink extsim ratio-dependent niche white extbasalnon pink dominance ratio-dependent random white dominance pink extbasalnon average entropy prey-dependent niche white dominance pink dominance prey-dependent random white dominance pink dominance ratio-dependent niche white eprey pink dominance ratio-dependent random white dominance pink dominance power spectrum prey-dependent niche white dominance pink dominance prey-dependent random white dominance pink dominance ratio-dependent niche white extniche pink dominance ratio-dependent random white dominance pink dominance 84 Yangchen Lin Predictors of invasion impact. Ecosystem metric Model type Noise Root predictor community structure prey-dependent niche white invsim pink ilink prey-dependent random white invbasalnon pink imut ratio-dependent niche white ipred pink ipred ratio-dependent random white ipred pink invbasalnon cv energy prey-dependent niche white invniche pink invsim prey-dependent random white invniche pink ilink ratio-dependent niche white invsim pink iprey ratio-dependent random white invsim pink imut cv entropy prey-dependent niche white invniche pink invsim prey-dependent random white invniche pink invniche ratio-dependent niche white ipred pink iprey ratio-dependent random white invniche pink imut average energy prey-dependent niche white invniche pink imut prey-dependent random white imut pink ipred ratio-dependent niche white iprey pink invbasalnon ratio-dependent random white invbasalnon pink invbasalnon average entropy prey-dependent niche white invniche pink invsim prey-dependent random white iprey pink invniche ratio-dependent niche white invniche pink iprey ratio-dependent random white invbasalnon pink invbasalnon power spectrum prey-dependent niche white invniche pink invbasalnon prey-dependent random white ipred pink invsim ratio-dependent niche white ipred pink invniche ratio-dependent random white ipred pink ipred 85 Extinction & invasion do not add up Predictors of extinction reversibility. Ecosystem metric Model type Noise Root predictor community structure prey-dependent niche white elink pink eprey prey-dependent random white dominance pink extsim ratio-dependent niche white dominance pink dominance ratio-dependent random white dominance pink extsim cv energy prey-dependent niche white elink pink eprey prey-dependent random white dominance pink extsim ratio-dependent niche white extsim pink elink ratio-dependent random white dominance pink extniche cv entropy prey-dependent niche white elink pink eprey prey-dependent random white dominance pink extsim ratio-dependent niche white extsim pink elink ratio-dependent random white dominance pink epred average energy prey-dependent niche white elink pink eprey prey-dependent random white elink pink extsim ratio-dependent niche white dominance pink elink ratio-dependent random white dominance pink dominance average entropy prey-dependent niche white elink pink eprey prey-dependent random white dominance pink extsim ratio-dependent niche white dominance pink dominance ratio-dependent random white dominance pink epred power spectrum prey-dependent niche white elink pink eprey prey-dependent random white elink pink extsim ratio-dependent niche white dominance pink dominance ratio-dependent random white dominance pink extsim 86 Yangchen Lin Predictors of invasion reversibility. Ecosystem metric Model type Noise Root predictor community structure prey-dependent niche white invsim pink iprey prey-dependent random white ilink pink invsim ratio-dependent niche white invniche pink invsim ratio-dependent random white iprey pink ipred cv energy prey-dependent niche white invsim pink iprey prey-dependent random white ilink pink invsim ratio-dependent niche white invniche pink invniche ratio-dependent random white ipred pink invniche cv entropy prey-dependent niche white invniche pink iprey prey-dependent random white ilink pink invsim ratio-dependent niche white invniche pink invniche ratio-dependent random white imut pink iprey average energy prey-dependent niche white invniche pink iprey prey-dependent random white ilink pink invniche ratio-dependent niche white invniche pink invniche ratio-dependent random white ipred pink invniche average entropy prey-dependent niche white invsim pink iprey prey-dependent random white ilink pink invniche ratio-dependent niche white invniche pink invniche ratio-dependent random white ipred pink invniche power spectrum prey-dependent niche white invniche pink iprey prey-dependent random white ilink pink imut ratio-dependent niche white invniche pink invsim ratio-dependent random white ipred pink ilink 87 Extinction & invasion do not add up Predictors of extinction-invasion impact. Ecosystem metric Model type Noise Root predictor community structure prey-dependent niche white extsim pink trocon prey-dependent random white iesim pink dominance ratio-dependent niche white trocon pink ipred ratio-dependent random white trocon pink trocon cv energy prey-dependent niche white eprey pink extsim prey-dependent random white trocon pink extsim ratio-dependent niche white invsim pink dominance ratio-dependent random white invsim pink imut cv entropy prey-dependent niche white eprey pink eprey prey-dependent random white invsim pink extsim ratio-dependent niche white extniche pink dominance ratio-dependent random white invsim pink epred average energy prey-dependent niche white eprey pink imut prey-dependent random white iesim pink trocon ratio-dependent niche white extbasalnon pink dominance ratio-dependent random white invbasalnon pink dominance average entropy prey-dependent niche white dominance pink dominance prey-dependent random white dominance pink dominance ratio-dependent niche white dominance pink dominance ratio-dependent random white dominance pink dominance power spectrum prey-dependent niche white dominance pink dominance prey-dependent random white dominance pink dominance ratio-dependent niche white extniche pink invniche ratio-dependent random white dominance pink dominance 88 Yangchen Lin Predictors of extinction-invasion interaction. Ecosystem metric Model type Noise Root predictor community structure prey-dependent niche white ipred pink extsim prey-dependent random white trocon pink extniche ratio-dependent niche white trocon pink ipred ratio-dependent random white dominance pink epred cv energy prey-dependent niche white dominance pink eprey prey-dependent random white invniche pink extniche ratio-dependent niche white ipred pink dominance ratio-dependent random white dominance pink imut cv entropy prey-dependent niche white dominance pink eprey prey-dependent random white invniche pink extniche ratio-dependent niche white ipred pink dominance ratio-dependent random white dominance pink invsim average energy prey-dependent niche white emut pink epred prey-dependent random white ilink pink extsim ratio-dependent niche white invniche pink extsim ratio-dependent random white mutcon pink invniche average entropy prey-dependent niche white dominance pink dominance prey-dependent random white imut pink trocon ratio-dependent niche white imut pink invniche ratio-dependent random white mutcon pink dominance power spectrum prey-dependent niche white dominance pink dominance prey-dependent random white dominance pink dominance ratio-dependent niche white emut pink invniche ratio-dependent random white dominance pink dominance 89 Extinction & invasion do not add up 4.5.2 effects of community structure Kendall  correlations between impacts on community structure and impacts on the other ecosystem metrics. Response variables were converted to absolute values before statistical testing. Asterisked parentheses *( )* indicate p < where = 0:01 after Bonferroni correction for five ecosystem metrics. EXTINCTION IMPACT Model type Noise cv . energy cv . entropy av . energy av . entropy powspec prey - dep . niche white *(0.412)* *(0.425)* *(0.538)* *(0.422)* -0.0653 pink *(0.401)* *(0.304)* *(0.489)* *(0.32)* -0.0781 prey -dep . random white *(0.517)* *(0.469)* *(0.607)* *(0.419)* *( -0.191)* pink *(0.439)* *(0.378)* *(0.564)* *(0.359)* -0.149 ratio - dep . niche white *(0.35)* *(0.26)* *(0.399)* 0.0812 -0.176 pink *(0.271)* 0.111 *(0.447)* 0.171 *( -0.206)* ratio - dep . random white *(0.585)* *(0.367)* *(0.53)* *(0.347)* *( -0.355)* pink *(0.247)* 0.159 *(0.5)* 0.182 *( -0.295)* INVASION IMPACT Model type Noise cv . energy cv . entropy av . energy av . entropy powspec prey - dep . niche white *(0.437)* *(0.379)* *(0.424)* *(0.234)* 0.0855 pink *(0.409)* *(0.298)* *(0.453)* *(0.18)* 0.107 prey -dep . random white *(0.533)* *(0.489)* *(0.644)* *(0.495)* 0.0421 pink *(0.461)* *(0.454)* *(0.54)* *(0.408)* 0.0481 ratio - dep . niche white *(0.297)* *(0.189)* *(0.484)* 0.117 *( -0.309)* pink *(0.211)* 0.127 *(0.367)* 0.028 *( -0.233)* ratio - dep . random white *(0.569)* *(0.498)* *(0.659)* *(0.423)* *( -0.342)* pink *(0.495)* *(0.357)* *(0.701)* *(0.55)* *( -0.225)* EXTINCTION-INVASION IMPACT Model type Noise cv . energy cv . entropy av . energy av . entropy powspec prey - dep . niche white *(0.396)* *(0.429)* *(0.462)* *(0.383)* 0.07 pink *(0.309)* *(0.204)* *(0.35)* *(0.226)* 0.0667 prey -dep . random white *(0.389)* *(0.291)* *(0.474)* *(0.272)* 0.00789 pink *(0.305)* *(0.372)* *(0.463)* *(0.364)* -0.105 ratio - dep . niche white *(0.279)* *(0.257)* *(0.22)* 0.145 -0.113 pink 0.133 -0.00728 0.137 0.121 -0.188 ratio - dep . random white *(0.387)* *(0.246)* *(0.336)* 0.0931 -0.129 pink *(0.226)* 0.0544 *(0.341)* 0.0739 -0.0595 90 Yangchen Lin 4.5.3 effects of extinctor dominance ● ● ●● ●● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●●● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● Extinctor dominance N o . o f p re d a to r sp ec ie s 0.0 0.4 0.8 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 a ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● Extinctor dominance E x t in c t o r n ic h e v a lu e 0.0 0.4 0.8 0.0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 b Relationships between extinctor dominance and (a) number of predators and (b) niche value. Pooled across all models (a); pooled across niche-topology models (b). 91 5 collective effect of interaction modifications Abstract. Interaction modification (im), where one species modifies the strength of the density-mediated direct interaction between two other species, is an important ecological process, but little is known about the collective effect of multiple im on overall community dynamics. I use stochastic bioenergetic modeling of ecological networks with differ- ent network topologies, functional responses and parameter values, to investigate the effects of im connectance and im strength on ecosystem properties including the evenness of species abundances and variability of system biomass. It was found that the maximum system biomass observed across the model systems increased with im connectance and strength when the models had nonrandom topology and prey-dependent functional responses as opposed to random topology and ratio-dependent responses. The maximum observed species evenness increased with im strength but decreased with increasing im connectance, when all modifications were negative. These findings underscore the importance of accounting for multiple im across the community for understanding complex community dynamics. 5.1 introduction Indirect effects are widely recognized as important drivers of the dynamics of ecological communities, and are a subject of active empirical and theoretical research [40, 351, 352]. There are two main types of indirect effects, acting via interaction chains and interaction modifications [353]. In an interaction chain, one species can indirectly affect the population density of a target species via density-mediated interactions with one or more intermediary species, such as in exploitative competition and trophic cascades [354], rather than direct interaction. On the other hand, an interaction modification [355], also known by the jargonologies of ‘trait- mediated indirect interaction’ [356], ‘non-consumptive effect’ [357] and ‘rheagogy’ [358], refers fundamentally to one species affecting the target 93 Collective effect of interaction modifications species by modulating the latter’s interactions with other species. Both types of indirect effects have been found in both empirical and theoretical studies to vary widely in their impacts on the stability and relative species abundances of ecological communities, depending on the relative strengths of direct and indirect effects and the quantitative balance between positive and negative feedback loops in ecological networks [354, 359]. This chapter focuses on interaction modification (im), which is also a useful implicit representation of behaviour [352]. The prevalence of im in natural communities has been recognized in recent years. For example, predator modification of prey grazing has been shown to be a mechanism accounting for much of the observed variation in the strength of trophic cascades [360], and im has been suggested as an alternative way to look at host-parasite interactions, where the parasite affects the feeding efficiency of the host [156]. Some mutualisms commonly formalized as pairwise interactions are actually part of tripartite im, such as bipartite networks of ant-plant mutualisms where ants mediate plant susceptibility to herbivory, or fish protected from predation while living amidst and contributing nutrients to sessile marine organisms [163, 168]. Werner & Peacor [356] reviewed the mechanistic basis and empirical evidence for the presence of im in both aquatic and terrestrial ecosystems and found that im were widespread in ecosystems and often as strong as or stronger than density-mediated effects. The importance of embracing im in understanding the response of real-world communities to global change has recently been highlighted [342], especially since im may play a role in engendering alternative stable states [355]. Bolker et al. [361] found that most of the early theoretical work on the effects of im on community dynamics pertained mostly to the mathematical stability of simple communities and that actual dynamics depended on details that were rarely measured empirically, such as func- tional responses. An ecosystem-level understanding also requires a grasp of the collective effects of the multiple im present in a given community, but most research has involved analysis of one im in a particular com- munity (e.g. Hsieh & Perfecto [351]). There is little knowledge on either 94 Yangchen Lin the overall topology and distribution of strengths of im in the entire community, or how multiple im collectively affect ecosystem properties. Multiple im present in a community may interact antagonistically with one another to reduce the total im effect on a given direct interaction, but little is known about the detailed relationships [40]. There is also evidence for cascading im, where a species modifies the feeding activities of another species that, in turn, modifies the density-mediated direct interaction between yet another two species [362], but its implications at the ecosystem level are unknown. Theoretical studies have begun to show that incorporating im can have a range of ecosystem effects. In models with four trophic levels, the presence of positive im gave rise to increased efficiency of abiotic nutrient use [358]. Goudard & Loreau [251] showed that greater connectance or magnitude of im in interaction web models with three distinct trophic levels tended to reduce species richness, biomass and production. More recently, Kamran-Disfani & Golubski [352] found that different kinds of im-related adaptive behaviour can either facilitate or inhibit the propagation of disturbances through food webs with three or four distinct trophic levels and that the effects also depended on food web structure. Although they examined each behaviour separately, they proposed that future models should incorporate multiple types of im (representing both foraging and defence) simultaneously. The incorporation of im in models of entire ecological communities is still in its infancy; most bioenergetic models of food webs, which represent the state of the art in dynamic community modeling, do not incorporate im. Models that have incorporated im are often simplified in terms of network topology, such as having a small number of trophic levels with no omnivory or looping [251, 352]. I investigate the collective effects of different degrees of im on various ecosystem-level properties of bioenergetic ecological network models with realistic network structure and metabolically scaled community dynamics. I compare the responses of models with random and nonrandom network topologies and also with prey- and ratio-dependent functional responses, 95 Collective effect of interaction modifications while incorporating realistic partial synchrony of species responses to pink environmental noise. A wide range of ecologically plausible parameter values are explored. This approach aims to clarify how im may influence the macroscopic properties of complex ecological communities in noisy real-world environments, with potential implications for their predictive modeling and management. 5.2 methods Models were based on the specifications in Chapter 2 and categorized into four possible configurations of trophic network topology and functional response: (1) topology based on the niche model of food webs combined with prey-dependent consumer-resource functional responses, (2) random topology with prey dependence, (3) niche topology with ratio dependence and (4) random topology with ratio dependence. All networks comprised 25 species. Parasitism and direct basal competition were omitted for comparability with random-topology models. Pink environmental noise with partial synchrony of species responses (§2.3) was used, as this was considered the most realistic. The prey-dependent consumer-resource functional response was mod- ified from Eqn. 2.2a following the im implementation of Goudard & Loreau [251]) and is given by Φi;j = Mi;j!i;jEj h 0:5h + ∑ k∈resources Mi;k!i;kEk h (5.1) with resource preference ! and Hill exponent h as before. The additional term M is defined as Mi;j = S∏ k=1 (1 + Ek) i;j;k , (5.2) being the total effect on the interaction between species i and j caused by all species that modify that interaction,  being the strength of interaction modification by a given species k [251]. This formulation of 96 Yangchen Lin im satisfies basic ecological requirements [251], such as increasing with the modifier species’ biomass, having no effect when the biomass is zero, and giving the same direction of energy flow regardless of the sign of the modification. The corresponding ratio-dependent functional response is given by Φi;j = Mi;j!i;j(Ej=Ei) h 1h + ∑ k∈resources Mi;k!i;k(Ek=Ei) h . (5.3) Although im, as implemented by Goudard & Loreau [251], can be both antagonistic and facilitative, it is still important to retain in the model facilitative interactions that depend directly on the states of the interacting species, as this is fundamentally different from im [353]. Kéfi et al. [41] recommended having both of them in dynamical models of ecological networks. There is little empirical knowledge of real topologies of im in eco- logical networks. The topology of im across the network was therefore assigned randomly; any type of species could influence the interaction between one or more species pairs of any type, representing all pos- sible types of adaptive behaviour—foraging, defence or otherwise (see Dambacher & Ramos-Jiliberto [355] p. 231). Furthermore, a modifier species can concurrently interact directly with one or both of the species whose mutual interaction it modifies (see de Roode et al. [363] for an empirical example). Cannibalistic links were excluded from im. im connectance is defined in this study as Goudard & Loreau’s ‘non- trophic connectance’ [251]: the number of realized interaction modifications divided by the total number of possible interaction modifications, where number of possible modifications is the total number of species multiplied by the number of trophic links, minus two per link to account for the fact that the two species interacting via a given link cannot modify their own interaction. The im connectance was varied from 0 to 1 [251] in increments of 0:1, and the calculated number of im links was rounded to the nearest integer. I examined the effects of magnitude and spread 97 Collective effect of interaction modifications of im strengths within each level of im connectance. Two separate sim- ulation experiments were conducted, one looking at different range sizes (hereafter ‘spread’) of im strengths around mean 0, the other looking at different magnitudes of negative im strengths. In the first experiment,  was drawn from a uniform distribution of mean 0 and range size from [−0:1; 0:1] to [−0:5; 0:5] in increments of 0:1 on each side of the mean (Goudard & Loreau [251] used [−0:2; 0:2]). In the second experiment,  was drawn from a uniform distribution of range [−0:5;−0:4] to [−0:1; 0:0] in increments of 0:1. Positive magnitudes were not used because they were often found to give ecologically unrealistic behaviour, with species undergoing ‘runaway’ population growth to extreme values. For both experiments, a set of 100 control simulations was done with no im. For each combination of model configuration, im connectance and im strength, 100 independent model realizations and simulations were executed. The models were simulated for 20 000 time steps, and time steps 10 001 to 20 000 were extracted for analysis, having allowed initial transients to pass and all species to either reach steady state or be close to steady state. Pilot simulations indicated that the extracted time window encompassed all the main features and periodicities of the stochastic behaviour. Four ecosystem metrics were measured from the community time series as response variables: the means and standard deviations (sd) of system energy and entropy over time. Although a measure of the effective number of species [273], the entropy reduces here to a measure of species evenness, since the total number of species is constant in this study. Unlike previous chapters, the coefficients of variation were not extracted here, because I wished to distinguish between systems with high mean and variability and those with low mean and low variability, both of which would result in similar coefficients of variation. In addition to looking at the energy and entropy metrics individually, I also examined via the evenness-variability (mean entropy-sd energy) relationship, which is one way of looking at the diversity-stability relationship. Higher variability is interpreted as lower stability. 98 Yangchen Lin 5.3 results Simulations were discarded where extreme species behaviour resulted in computational infinity values caused by high positive im strength (also responsible for a few very large data points discussed below), and occasionally where negative species states were produced due to the noise terms and intrinsic numerical integration errors. The sample size of each treatment ranged from 97 to 100. Overall, there was little difference between the control simulations with no im and those with the lowest connectance and lowest spread and magnitude of im, for all of the response variables; the control experiment is therefore omitted from the analysis. 99 Collective effect of interaction m odifications 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 0 2 4 6 8 10 IM connectance IM sp rea d 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 0 2 4 6 8 10 IM connectance IM sp rea d 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 0 2 4 6 8 10 IM connectance IM sp rea d 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 0 2 4 6 8 10 IM connectance IM sp rea d 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 0 2 4 6 8 10 IM connectance IM sp rea d 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 0 2 4 6 8 10 IM connectance IM sp rea d 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 0 2 4 6 8 10 IM connectance IM sp rea d 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 0 2 4 6 8 10 IM connectance IM sp rea d l o g m a x m e a n e n e r g y l o g m a x S D e n e r g y Niche topo, prey-dep. Random topo, prey-dep. Niche topo, ratio-dep. Random topo, ratio-dep. Figure 5.1. Effect of im connectance and im spread on maximum observed means (upper row) and sd (lower row) of system energy. The surface vertex at each connectance-spread setting denotes the mean response of the models with the highest 5% of response values among about 100 independent replicate models with the given connectance and spread. The four main model configurations are segregated by column. 100 Y angchen L in 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 −0.4 −0.3 −0.2 −0.1 0 1 2 IM connectance IM m ean 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 −0.4 −0.3 −0.2 −0.1 0 1 2 IM connectance IM m ean 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 −0.4 −0.3 −0.2 −0.1 0 1 2 IM connectance IM m ean 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 −0.4 −0.3 −0.2 −0.1 0 1 2 IM connectance IM m ean 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 −0.4 −0.3 −0.2 −0.1 0 1 2 IM connectance IM m ean 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 −0.4 −0.3 −0.2 −0.1 0 1 2 IM connectance IM m ean 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 −0.4 −0.3 −0.2 −0.1 0 1 2 IM connectance IM m ean 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 −0.4 −0.3 −0.2 −0.1 0 1 2 IM connectance IM m ean l o g m a x m e a n e n e r g y l o g m a x S D e n e r g y Niche topo, prey-dep. Random topo, prey-dep. Niche topo, ratio-dep. Random topo, ratio-dep. Figure 5.2. Effect of im connectance and mean im magnitude on maximum observed means (upper row) and sd (lower row) of system energy. The surface vertex at each im connectance-mean setting denotes the mean response of the models with the highest 5% of response values among about 100 independent replicate models with the given im connectance and im mean. The four main model configurations are segregated by column. 101 Collective effect of interaction modifications In the ‘spread’ experiment where both positive and negative im are present, the maximum mean energy over time, as observed across all models, increases dramatically with both im connectance and strength for niche-topology prey-dependent models (Fig. 5.1, note log-transformed data), while the other three model configurations exhibit much less change, if at all. All model configurations show limited influence by all-negative im (Fig. 5.2). In both experiments, the minimum observed energy changes much more slowly, if at all (Figs. 5.5, 5.6 in Appendix 5.5.1). The sd of system energy over time also increases with im connectance and strength for niche-topology prey-dependent models (Figs. 5.1, 5.2). Ratio-dependent models also have lower maximum observed mean and sd energy, and are appreciably less variable in these maxima, across im connectances and strengths compared to prey-dependent models (Figs. 5.1, 5.2). The mean and sd of system energy are strongly positively correlated across the board (Fig. 5.7 in Appendix 5.5.1) as both are caused by high production rates leading to greater tendencies of cycles of population overshoot and collapse. 102 Y angchen L in 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 −0.4 −0.3 −0.2 −0.1 10 15 20 IM connectance IM m ean 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 −0.4 −0.3 −0.2 −0.1 10 15 20 IM connectance IM m ean 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 −0.4 −0.3 −0.2 −0.1 10 15 20 IM connectance IM m ean 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 −0.4 −0.3 −0.2 −0.1 10 15 20 IM connectance IM m ean a m a x m e a n e n t r o p y m a x m e a n e n t r o p y m a x m e a n e n t r o p y m a x m e a n e n t r o p y Niche topo, prey-dep. Random topo, prey-dep. Niche topo, ratio-dep. Random topo, ratio-dep. 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 −0.4 −0.3 −0.2 −0.1 5 10 IM connectance IM m ean 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 −0.4 −0.3 −0.2 −0.1 5 10 IM connectance IM m ean 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 −0.4 −0.3 −0.2 −0.1 5 10 IM connectance IM m ean 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 −0.4 −0.3 −0.2 −0.1 5 10 IM connectance IM m ean b m i n m e a n e n t r o p y m i n m e a n e n t r o p y m i n m e a n e n t r o p y m i n m e a n e n t r o p y Niche topo, prey-dep. Random topo, prey-dep. Niche topo, ratio-dep. Random topo, ratio-dep. Figure 5.3. Effect of im connectance and mean im magnitude on maximum (a) and minimum (b) observed mean entropies, for different model configurations of topology and functional response. The surface vertex at each im connectance-mean setting denotes the mean response of the models with the highest (a) or lowest (b) 5% of response values among about 100 independent replicate models with the given im connectance and im mean. 103 Collective effect of interaction modifications The maximum observed mean species evenness decreases when all im in a given system are increasingly negative and of increasing connectance (Fig. 5.3a). Niche-topology ratio-dependent models have on average the highest maximum observed mean evenness for any given im connectance and magnitude. The minimum observed mean evenness shows little or no systematic change, with niche-topology ratio-dependent models having on average considerably higher minimum evenness than the other model configurations (Fig. 5.3b). Models with random topology tend to have lower evenness than models with niche topology, for the same im connectance and strength (Fig. 5.3a). The sd of evenness shows little or no change with increasing im negativity and connectance; this is also the case when both negative and positive ims are present (all Pearson |/| and Kendall | | < 0:1). ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● Niche topo PD 10−2 100 102 104 106 108 1010 ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● Random topo PD ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ●●● ● ● ●● ●● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ●● ● ●● ●● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●●●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●●● ● ●●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●●● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ●● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● Niche topo RD ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ●● ●● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ●●● ● ●● ●● ● ●● ● ● ●● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● Random topo RD ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● 0 5 10 15 20 25 10−3 10−2 10−1 100 101 102 ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● 0 5 10 15 20 25 ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ●●●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●●●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ●●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● 0 5 10 15 20 25 ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● 0 5 10 15 20 25 a b Species evenness (mean exp[Shannon entropy]) V ar ia b il it y ( S D s y st em e n er gy ) Figure 5.4. Relationships between species evenness and system variability, by model configuration (columns). Row a, both positive and negative im co-occurring; row b, only negative im present. Data pooled across im connectances and strength levels. PD, Prey-Dependent; RD, Ratio-Dependent. 104 Yangchen Lin Evenness-variability relationship. Prey-dependent models exhibit two clus- ters of data points with opposite relationships, while ratio-dependent models have single clusters (weak to moderately strong positive Kendall  , most p < 0:05) resembling the lower variability-higher evenness clusters of the prey-dependent models with the respective niche or random network topologies (Fig. 5.4). Inspection of simulated species trajectories of prey- dependent models showed that systems with both very low evenness and very high variability tended to be characterized by one or two species having considerably higher energy states than the other species and continuing to increase monotonically in energy within the measurement window for the response variables while the other species had reached steady state. These simulations constituted the minority of the sample and were not omitted because their behaviour merges gradually with those in which all species reached steady state, with no thresholds of evenness or variability. The data clusters with opposite relationships occur regardless of im settings and therefore appear to be unrelated to im (see Appendix 5.5.2 for plots of unpooled data). 5.4 discussion The implementation of im by Goudard & Loreau [251] used in this study, with randomization of interaction modification strengths, is considered by Golubski & Abrams [40] as an ‘extreme simplification’. The overall formulation, however, is sufficient for studying the fundamental effects of im compared with no modification, across contingencies represented by the randomization of the magnitudes and directions of im strengths. Olff et al. [82] suggest further types of indirect interactions that could be examined in future work. When negative and positive im are equally abundant, they tend to cancel out one another’s effects on maximum observed entropy and maximum observed system energy as expected, dampening the effects of im connectance and strength except for the energy of prey-dependent models with niche topology (Fig. 5.1). The exceptional behaviour of the prey-dependent models may be partly because predators at higher trophic 105 Collective effect of interaction modifications levels can have many prey species in a system with niche topology, and can increase to particularly high abundances by collectively depressing their numerous diet species to low-energy steady states when their functional responses are only prey-dependent and enhanced by strong positive im. In this case the negative im do not cancel out the positive im because the potential population growth due to positive im has no upper bound while population reduction due to negative im has a lower bound of zero. In contrast to niche topology and prey dependence, random topology averages out the number of prey species per predator, and ratio dependence regulates predator population growth more tightly with or without im by making the predator and prey populations fluctuate proportionately (see Arditi & Ginzburg [180]). Nevertheless, caution must be exercised in comparing prey and ratio dependence in terms of quantitative absolute values, because the half-saturation parameter value used in the functional response is different and because of the fundamental structural differences in the models. In addition, strong and abundant negative modifications are associated with lower maximum observed evenness (Fig. 5.3a). This could be due to negative im weakening the direct interaction strengths between species, causing their abundances to be less dependent on one another, everything else being equal. Interestingly, models with random topology have lower maximum observed evenness (Fig. 5.3a). This may be related to the random links between the metabolically scaled species rate equations giving rise to a higher proportion of consumers with smaller niche values (body sizes) than their resources, giving rise to a higher incidence of deviations from the smooth energy pyramid. Such a scenario may apply to real systems in which parasite diversity is on par with host diversity. In contrast, niche-topology ratio-dependent models have higher maximum observed evenness on average, and considerably higher minimum observed evenness on average, than all the other model configurations (Fig. 5.3b). This could be due to ratio dependence mandating a more even distribution of energy among predator and prey because of their proportionate population change, combined with the aforementioned energy-pyramid 106 Yangchen Lin effect. Along with the results for system energy, the results for evenness lean towards niche topology-ratio dependence as a high evenness-low variability configuration that may be beneficial in terms of ecosystem function and robustness. For all im settings examined, the evenness-variability relationship is generally higher evenness-higher variability for ratio-dependent models while prey-dependent models are split into two clusters with opposite relationships, the high evenness-low variability cluster representing those systems that have reached steady state and resembling the relationship for ratio-dependent models (Fig. 5.4). For ratio dependence especially, the results appear to be in conflict with the current consensus on biodiversity generally having stabilizing effects on various kinds of stability, including stability in terms of ecosystem variability [241]. The solution to this paradox may be that my measure of ‘diversity’ is restricted to the species evenness component: when species abundances are more evenly distributed, species are on average at greater liberty to ‘explore’ their dynamical state spaces, as contrasted with a highly uneven system heavily dominated by one species that monopolizes most of the resources leaving very little leeway for the others. Indeed, the latter kind of system simulated in this study may represent real systems under siege by invasive species or threatened by increasing activity by the human species. This study therefore provides fresh insights on the diversity-stability relationship, given that most studies have considered the number of species but not their relative abundances. Notably, my results seem to depart qualitatively from those of Goudard & Loreau [251] in terms of the maximum observed mean system energy being positively associated or not associated with im connectance and spread, depending on functional response (Fig. 5.1), opposing the rela- tionship found by Goudard & Loreau for equilibrium biomass. Possible factors include the use of more realistic network topology, functional responses and stochasticity, and the lack of explicit nutrient limitation, in my model; further work may shed light on how important these factors are for building realistic ecological models. 107 Collective effect of interaction modifications In reality, im are most likely to be antagonistic to each other [40] such that multiple positive im are collectively less positive, and negative im less negative, than their sum, but little is known empirically. My study assumes, as in Goudard & Loreau [251], that the strengths of multiple im are independent of one another, such that increasing the im connectance causes the total im strength to increase additively. This may lead to overestimation of the effect sizes, but I argue that the findings will be qualitatively similar. This is because it is likely that the total im strength would still accumulate in nature with increasing im connectance, only that it would be less than purely additive. In addition to such ‘density dependence’ of combined im strength, the variation of im strength over time i.e. adaptive trait dynamics [364] also needs better understanding. In conclusion, although individual im have been reported to have limited impact on food webs [352], I found for a wide range of parameter values that the multiple im that would be present in a real ecosystem can have ecosystem-level effects depending on the strengths and connectance of those modifications, but the effects pertain mostly to the potential maximum boundary conditions rather than those in which most of the model ecosystems reside. This suggests that multiple im in a community tend to cancel out one another’s effects under average conditions, supporting the view of Golubski & Abrams [40] that models that lack im may still give ‘adequate predictions’ for more speciose systems, in which im tend to have smaller net effects. In addition to elucidating the effects of im, the model behaviours observed in my study also underscore the importance of modulating the balance between prey- and ratio-dependent functional responses appropriately. Prey-dependent models are much more frequently used and, if used in food webs, most commonly combined with niche topology. In such cases, model behaviour may exhibit higher productivity and variability and lower evenness than reality, if the community were actually more ratio-dependent. Finally, the study of im can also provide insights into the role of ecosystem engineers, or species that physically modify their environment. One of the effects of ecosystem engineers is to ‘modulate’ the links between 108 Yangchen Lin other species via the environment [365]; this process is essentially im. Ecosystem engineering has been investigated only in simple food webs, and much remains to be discovered about its influence on larger and more complex ecological networks [365]. 109 Collective effect of interaction modifications 5.5 appendix 5.5.1 supplementary figures 110 Y angchen L in 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 −1.5 −1.0 −0.5 0.0 0.5 IM connectance IM sp rea d 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 −1.5 −1.0 −0.5 0.0 0.5 IM connectance IM sp rea d 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 −1.5 −1.0 −0.5 0.0 0.5 IM connectance IM sp rea d 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 −1.5 −1.0 −0.5 0.0 0.5 IM connectance IM sp rea d 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 −1.5 −1.0 −0.5 0.0 0.5 IM connectance IM sp rea d 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 −1.5 −1.0 −0.5 0.0 0.5 IM connectance IM sp rea d 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 −1.5 −1.0 −0.5 0.0 0.5 IM connectance IM sp rea d 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 −1.5 −1.0 −0.5 0.0 0.5 IM connectance IM sp rea d l o g m i n m e a n e n e r g y l o g m i n S D e n e r g y Niche topo, prey-dep. Random topo, prey-dep. Niche topo, ratio-dep. Random topo, ratio-dep. Figure 5.5. Effect of im connectance and im spread on minimum observed means (upper row) and sd (lower row) of system energy. The surface vertex at each connectance-spread setting denotes the mean response of the models with the lowest 5% of response values among about 100 independent replicate models with the given connectance and spread. 111 Collective effect of interaction m odifications 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 −0.4 −0.3 −0.2 −0.1 −2 −1 0 IM connectance IM m ean 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 −0.4 −0.3 −0.2 −0.1 −2 −1 0 IM connectance IM m ean 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 −0.4 −0.3 −0.2 −0.1 −2 −1 0 IM connectance IM m ean 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 −0.4 −0.3 −0.2 −0.1 −2 −1 0 IM connectance IM m ean 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 −0.4 −0.3 −0.2 −0.1 −2 −1 0 IM connectance IM m ean 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 −0.4 −0.3 −0.2 −0.1 −2 −1 0 IM connectance IM m ean 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 −0.4 −0.3 −0.2 −0.1 −2 −1 0 IM connectance IM m ean 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 −0.4 −0.3 −0.2 −0.1 −2 −1 0 IM connectance IM m ean l o g m i n m e a n e n e r g y l o g m i n S D e n e r g y Niche topo, prey-dep. Random topo, prey-dep. Niche topo, ratio-dep. Random topo, ratio-dep. Figure 5.6. Effect of im connectance and mean im magnitude on minimum observed means (upper row) and sd (lower row) of system energy. The surface vertex at each im connectance-mean setting denotes the mean response of the models with the lowest 5% of response values among about 100 independent replicate models with the given im connectance and im mean. 112 Yangchen Lin ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● Niche topo PD 10−2 100 102 104 106 108 1010 ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● Random topo PD ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●●● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ●● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● Niche topo RD ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ●● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● Random topo RD ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● 0.0 1.0 2.0 10−3 10−2 10−1 100 101 102 ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● 0.0 1.0 2.0 ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● 0.0 1.0 2.0 ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ●●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● 0.0 1.0 2.0 a b log mean system energy V ar ia b il it y ( S D s y st em e n er gy ) Figure 5.7. Relationships between mean and sd of system energy, by model con- figuration (columns). Row a, both positive and negative im co-occurring; row b, only negative im present. Data pooled across im connectances and strength levels. PD, Prey-Dependent; RD, Ratio-Dependent. 5.5.2 plots of unpooled data Relationships between species evenness and system variability, plotted separately for each im connectance and strength level within each model configuration within each simulation experiment (see §5.2 for methodolog- ical details). Axes same as Fig. 5.4 in main text. Axis limits are different (maximized) in each subplot. 113 Collective effect of interaction m odifications ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●●●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ●● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ●●● ●● ● 1.0 ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●●● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● 0.8 ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● V a r i a b i l i t y ( l o g S D s y s t e m e n e r g y ) ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●●●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● 0.6 IM sp read ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● 0.4 ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ●●● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ●●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● 0.2 Species evenness (mean exp[Shannon entropy]) IM connectance 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5 0.6 0.7 0.8 0.9 1.0 im spread experiment: niche-topology prey-dependent models 114 Y angchen L in ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ●● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ●● ●●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ●●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ●● ● ●●●● ● 1.0 ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●●● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ●● ●●● ● ●● ●●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●●● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● 0.8 ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● V a r i a b i l i t y ( l o g S D s y s t e m e n e r g y ) ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●●●●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● 0.6 IM sp read ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ●●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ●● ●●● ●● ● ●●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● 0.4 ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ●● ●● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● 0.2 Species evenness (mean exp[Shannon entropy]) IM connectance 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5 0.6 0.7 0.8 0.9 1.0 im spread experiment: random-topology prey-dependent models 115 Collective effect of interaction m odifications ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● 1.0 ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ●● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●●●● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ●●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●●●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● 0.8 ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● V a r i a b i l i t y ( l o g S D s y s t e m e n e r g y ) ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● 0.6 IM sp read ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●●● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● 0.4 ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● 0.2 Species evenness (mean exp[Shannon entropy]) IM connectance 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5 0.6 0.7 0.8 0.9 1.0 im spread experiment: niche-topology ratio-dependent models 116 Y angchen L in ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●●● ● ●● ● ●●●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●●● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● 1.0 ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●●● ● ●●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● 0.8 ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● V a r i a b i l i t y ( l o g S D s y s t e m e n e r g y ) ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● 0.6 IM sp read ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● 0.4 ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● 0.2 Species evenness (mean exp[Shannon entropy]) IM connectance 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5 0.6 0.7 0.8 0.9 1.0 im spread experiment: random-topology ratio-dependent models 117 Collective effect of interaction m odifications ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● − 0.05 ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ●●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ●●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● − 0.15 ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● V a r i a b i l i t y ( l o g S D s y s t e m e n e r g y ) ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●●● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● − 0.25 IM m ean ● ● ● ●●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ●● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ●●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● − 0.35 ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● − 0.45 Species evenness (mean exp[Shannon entropy]) IM connectance 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5 0.6 0.7 0.8 0.9 1.0 im magnitude experiment: niche-topology prey-dependent models 118 Y angchen L in ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ●● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ●●● ● ●● ● ● ●●● ●● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● − 0.05 ● ● ● ●●● ● ●● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ●● ●● ●● ●●● ●● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ●●●● ● ●● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ●● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● − 0.15 ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● V a r i a b i l i t y ( l o g S D s y s t e m e n e r g y ) ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●●● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ●● ● − 0.25 IM m ean ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ●● ●● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●●● ●● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ●●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●●● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● − 0.35 ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ●●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● − 0.45 Species evenness (mean exp[Shannon entropy]) IM connectance 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5 0.6 0.7 0.8 0.9 1.0 im magnitude experiment: random-topology prey-dependent models 119 Collective effect of interaction m odifications ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ●●● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● − 0.05 ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●●●● ● − 0.15 ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● V a r i a b i l i t y ( l o g S D s y s t e m e n e r g y ) ● ● ●● ●● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●●● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ●●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● − 0.25 IM m ean ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● − 0.35 ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ●●● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● − 0.45 Species evenness (mean exp[Shannon entropy]) IM connectance 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5 0.6 0.7 0.8 0.9 1.0 im magnitude experiment: niche-topology ratio-dependent models 120 Y angchen L in ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ●● ●● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● − 0.05 ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●●●● ● ● ● ●●● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● − 0.15 ●● ●●●● ●● ●●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●●● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● V a r i a b i l i t y ( l o g S D s y s t e m e n e r g y ) ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● − 0.25 IM m ean ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ●●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● − 0.35 ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ●● ●●● ● ●● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●●●● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ●● ● ● ●● ● ● ● − 0.45 Species evenness (mean exp[Shannon entropy]) IM connectance 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5 0.6 0.7 0.8 0.9 1.0 im magnitude experiment: random-topology ratio-dependent models 121 6 species decline in compartmentalized networks Abstract. The complex network approach is increasingly used to further our understanding and management of complex systems in a wide range of disciplines, including ecology. It has recently been recognized that networks are often compartmentalized, and that this can have important implications for their resilience to perturbations. I constructed stochastic bioenergetic models comprising two compartments each containing trophic and mutualistic interactions, with varying levels of intercompartment con- nectivity. After steady state was reached, I simulated species declines in one compartment and quantified their impacts on the biomass, diversity, community composition and time series power spectra of both compart- ments. Intercompartment trophic connectivity (density of feeding links between the compartments) was positively correlated with the impacts of species decline on the Shannon diversity of the compartments, but negatively correlated with the impacts on power spectra of species time series. Impacts on community composition were positively correlated with connectivity when the network topology was based on the niche model of food webs, but negatively correlated when the topology was random. In contrast with trophic connectivity, the effects of intercompartment mutualistic connectivity (density of mutualistic links) were much smaller. The results show that differing intercompartment connectivity in networks can cause the impacts of species decline to have different magnitudes and directions depending on network topology and the ecosystem property being measured. The variability found across replicate simulations also highlights the context dependence of ecosystem behaviour and suggests that caution should be exercised when applying conclusions from one specific system to other systems. 6.1 introduction In our increasingly interconnected world there is a growing interest in, and relevance of, the network approach to understanding the complex 123 Species decline in compartmentalized networks systems that pervade our environment, many of which have hitherto been studied in a reductionistic way, in disciplines as disparate as neuroscience and ecology. The scientific community is now appreciating the vulnerability of human-designed or human-impacted natural networks to catastrophic tipping points and need for multidisciplinary integrated global systems science to address such problems [366]. To this end of capturing the totality of interdependencies that govern the characteristics of real-world complexity, the study of ‘networks of networks’ has been hailed as the next frontier of complexity science [367, 368]. Real networks are usually highly compartmentalized [369, 370], ranging from the World Wide Web [371] to the brain connectome [372], to metabolic and signalling networks in cells [373, 374] and to ecological networks of interacting species [375]. Compartmentalization can have consequences for the behaviour or resilience of networks to perturbation [376, 377], such as by limiting the extent of cascades [26, 378]. There has been a corresponding development of network structural complexity metrics that account for compartments and their nonplanarity [379]. In ecological networks of interacting species, a compartment can be defined as some group of highly interconnected species that are highly interconnected with one another but connected relatively sparsely to other such groups. Pimm & Lawton [380] found little empirical evidence for compartmentalization in food webs within relatively homogeneous habitats. Since then, however, with more recent and more highly resolved data sets, it has been found that many kinds of ecological networks do exhibit compartmentalized topology, such as food webs [381, 382] and insect- plant interaction networks [383, 384]. Furthermore, compartmentalization is generally recognized to enhance stability and persistence in food webs [48, 290]. In this study, I consider two-compartment networks, each compartment being a spatially distinct ecological network that contains a different set of species. Examples include canopy, understorey and soil compartments in forest, and aquatic-terrestrial systems. Little research has been done on compartmentalization at this scale, most studies having looked at either 124 Yangchen Lin green or brown webs in isolation [385] or compartmentalization within what is actually a functional group within a larger ecological network, for example host-parasitoid communities in forest [262, 386]. Some recent empirical studies have demonstrated that intercompartment interactions at the habitat level play important roles in community dynamics. For example, Giery et al. [387] demonstrated the existence of trophic interactions and corresponding resource flux between canopy and understorey food webs in woods. Trophic links between above-ground and below-ground webs cause dynamic feedbacks [385], while those between elements of a spatial mosaic of food webs in a floodplain affect the carrying capacity and recovery of predator populations [388]. Facilitative interactions can also occur between spatial compartments. For example, waterbodies created by beavers support insects that, in turn, benefit terrestrial bats [389]. Very few studies have considered the explicit interaction topology of compartmentalized networks at the ecosystem level. Pocock et al. [116] found varying effects of species decline on the various subnetworks, Valiente-Banuet & Verdú [295] reported that anthropogenic impacts can act synergistically to cause network collapse, while Evans et al. [390] found that the loss of certain habitats has disproportionate effects on network integrity. These studies, however, did not investigate the effect of compartmentalization per se. Furthermore, each compartment in these studies collated a particular type of interaction of interest, rather than the actual co-occurrence of interactions of multiple types between all the closely interacting species in a given subhabitat, and considered only network topology without dynamics. The aforementioned studies provide important insights for specific systems that are likely to be highly context-dependent; there thus re- mains scope for more general theoretical understanding of the role of compartmentalization in the dynamics of complex ecological networks, as most models of ecological networks are not compartmentalized [391]. An exception is Krause et al. [375], who examined the effects of removing nodes from compartmentalized food webs constructed from empirical data, and found that compartmentalization limited the extent of cascades, re- 125 Species decline in compartmentalized networks sulting in greater stability. They, however, did not incorporate community dynamics, which can be an important factor in mediating the stability of the system [32]. Stouffer & Bascompte [290] came to similar conclusions with compartmentalized food web models that incorporated community dynamics, but did not incorporate nontrophic interactions or interaction modifications. Here, I use holistic models of ecological networks to investigate the simplest case of compartmentalization, in which two such networks are connected to each other by varying numbers of trophic and mutualistic links, or connectivity. I ask how differing levels of intercompartment trophic and mutualistic connectivity affect the impact and propagation of species declines in one compartment both on itself and on the other compartment. I assess the impact using an array of ecosystem metrics that are indicative of ecosystem health, as well as of broad interest, such as biomass, diversity and community composition. 6.2 methods The network models comprised two independently generated (Chapter 2) 15-species compartments linked to each other, making a total of 30 species in the system. In a given model realization, the two compartments had the same trophic topology, which was either random or based on the generalized niche model (see Chapter 2), and the niche range was kept constant at (0; 1) for both compartments. These are not ecologically unreasonable simplifying assumptions, even though differences between compartments have sometimes been observed, such as between forest soil and above-ground food webs [392]. Ratio-dependent consumer-resource functional responses were used. Interaction modification (Chapter 5) was confined to within compartments, with connectance 0:1 and modification strengths drawn from a uniform distribution in [−0:1; 0:1]. Environmental stochasticity was implemented as pink noise with partial interspecific synchrony (§2.3). As there is little empirical information on the actual topology of inter- compartment links, trophic and mutualistic links were randomly assigned 126 Yangchen Lin between the compartments. The intercompartment trophic and mutualistic connectivities were both varied from 0:05 to 0:3 in increments of 0:05 in a full factorial design, while the within-compartment trophic and mu- tualistic connectances were kept constant at 0:3, near the upper limit of the empirically observed range [158]. Assigning intercompartment links at random means that species most highly interconnected with one another (i.e. modules) may not correspond exactly to compartments in a given model realization. The two main modules in each model were therefore identified using the community detection algorithm in the igraph package [393] in R, and models in which the modules did not correspond exactly to the compartments were discarded. The directedness of the intercompartment trophic links was decided as follows: for niche-topology models, the species of higher niche value in a species pair would be the consumer if both were non-basal species; for random-topology models, one of the species was randomly assigned as consumer. If one of the pair was a basal species, it was fed on by the non-basal species regardless of niche value or network topology. Basal species pairs were excluded from trophic interactions. Finally, the Laplacian matrix of the entire network was computed, as in §2.1, to ensure that there were no isolated components. The models were simulated to 20 000 time steps to allow initial transients to pass, and a set of four ecosystem metrics was calculated using the simulation output from time steps 20 001 to 40 000 after steady state had been reached. The ecosystem metrics (§4.2) were the average total system energy over time, average exponent of Shannon entropy over time, community composition8 and power spectrum. From time step 40 001 onwards, five randomly selected species (hereafter ‘declinees’) in one compartment (hereafter ‘proximal compartment’, the other compartment referred to as ‘distal’) were subjected to chronic decline of 0.01 of the species states at each time step until the end of the simulation at 8This is the ‘community structure’ of previous chapters; the terminology is altered here to avoid confusion with the term of the same name in network science. 127 Species decline in compartmentalized networks time step 80 000.9 The decline rate was chosen such that its effect was considerably greater than that of the environmental noise. The same ecosystem metrics were recalculated post-decline using time steps 60 001 to 80 000. The impact of decline on each ecosystem metric was then calculated as detailed in §4.2, separately for each compartment. A sample of 128 independent model realizations was generated for each intercompartment trophic-mutualistic connectivity treatment. Another set of simulations and analyses was also run as already described, but with the declinees being non-basal species with the five highest niche values in the proximal compartment, representing the decline of large animals or top predators. Simulations were discarded where negative species states were pro- duced due to the noise terms and intrinsic numerical integration errors. In addition, extreme values occasionally occurred in impacts measured as proportional change, as explained in §4.3, resulting in infinity values via computer rounding error; these replicates were also taken out of the analysis. As stated earlier, models in which modules did not correspond to compartments were discarded. The final sample size for each intercom- partment trophic-mutualistic connectivity combination ranged from 63 to 128. Regression trees were constructed to identify the most important de- terminants of decline impact among the following: compartment topology (niche or random), niche range of the declinees in a given simulation (either the full niche range of the community or the top five predators only), intercompartment trophic and mutualistic connectivities, numbers of intercompartment trophic and mutualistic links associated with declinees, the number of declinees that are basal species, and what I term the average trophic centrality and average mutualistic centrality of declinees. The last two variables are defined as the average number of trophic and mutualistic links, respectively, that each declinee has in a given network realization; they are similar to degree centrality. The trophic centrality 9This implementation is more dynamically realistic than the instantaneous extinctions of pre- vious chapters, although it is not absolute extinction. 128 Yangchen Lin combines both predator and prey links to capture the essence of energy flow through the declinees. The main analysis then focuses on the relationships between inter- compartment connectivity and decline impacts on the various ecosystem metrics in the proximal and distal compartments. 6.3 results The regression trees (Appendix 6.6.1) show that network topology explains the greatest proportion of the deviance for all ecosystem metrics in both compartments. There are small correlations (all Kendall | | < 0:2, most p < 0:0125 after Bonferroni correction for four ecosystem metrics) between intercompartment trophic connectivity and impacts on proximal or distal compartments (Table 6.1); see below for details for each ecosystem metric. Correlations for intercompartment mutualistic connectivity are generally weaker (most | | < 0:1; Table 6.1). 129 Species decline in com partm entalized netw orks Table 6.1 (facing page). Kendall correlations between inter- compartment connectivity and decline impacts on ecosystem metrics. 130 Y angchen L in Compartment Ecosystem metric Declinee niche range Network topology .troph ptroph .mut pmut proximal average energy full niche −0:031 0:00457 −0:0786 6:06× 10−13 random 0:0389 0:000426 −0:0424 0:000121 high niche −0:106 1:57× 10−22 −0:121 1:04× 10−28 random 0:0379 0:000561 −0:0366 0:000852 average entropy full niche 0:1 0 −0:0786 5:95× 10−13 random 0:118 0 −0:0394 0:000356 high niche 0:198 0 −0:0869 1:31× 10−15 random 0:122 0 −0:0495 6:64× 10−6 community composition full niche 0:0926 0 0:0367 0:000785 random −0:0386 0:000464 0:0045 0:683 high niche 0:135 0 0:0543 5:89× 10−7 random −0:0467 2:11× 10−5 0:0281 0:0105 power spectrum full niche −0:119 1:52× 10−27 −0:0587 7:52× 10−8 random −0:162 1:04× 10−48 −0:0182 0:099 high niche −0:105 4:18× 10−22 −0:114 9:99× 10−26 random −0:165 9:31× 10−51 −0:0332 0:00249 distal average energy full niche −0:0705 1:06× 10−10 −0:0368 0:000737 random −0:032 0:00374 −0:00228 0:836 high niche −0:114 1:6 × 10−25 −0:0456 2:74× 10−5 random −0:0157 0:153 0:0112 0:307 average entropy full niche 0:046 2:48× 10−5 −0:0312 0:00427 random −0:0274 0:0129 −0:00451 0:683 high niche 0:0882 4:44× 10−16 −0:00776 0:476 random −0:0124 0:258 −0:000492 0:964 community composition full niche 0:12 0 0:0284 0:00919 random −0:03 0:00644 −0:0212 0:0547 high niche 0:131 0 0:0393 0:000302 random −0:032 0:00353 −0:027 0:0139 power spectrum full niche −0:0674 6:76× 10−10 0:0767 2:12× 10−12 random −0:0996 1:66× 10−19 −0:00258 0:815 high niche 0:00908 0:404 0:176 0 random −0:107 2:95× 10−22 −0:00177 0:872 Compartments—proximal: where declines happen; distal: the other compartment. Declinee niche ranges—full: random sample of all species in proximal compartment; high: species with five highest niche values. Subscripts: troph, intercompart- ment trophic connectivity; mut, intercompartment mutualistic connectivity. Significance level 5 (:()25 after Bonferroni correction for four ecosystem metrics. 131 Species decline in compartmentalized networks 0 20 40 60 80 100 120 N ic h e to p o lo g y −0.6 −0.2 0.2 0 20 40 60 80 100 120 −0.6 −0.2 0.2 −0.6 −0.2 0.2 −0.6 −0.2 0.2 −0.6 −0.2 0.2 −0.6 −0.2 0.2 R a n d o m to p o lo g y Impact on average system energy S im u la ti on s ra n ke d b y i m p ac t Intercompartment trophic connectivity levels 0.05 0.1 0.15 0.2 0.25 0.3 Figure 6.1. Decline impacts on average system energy for increasing intercom- partment trophic connectivity with intercompartment mutualistic connectivity (:(5. Results for other mutualistic connectivities are not illustrated as they show a similar pattern. In each panel, the x-axis is the impact on system energy and the y-axis is the rank of simulations sorted in increasing order of the impact on system energy in the proximal compartment, demarcated by the red curve. Impacts on the distal compartments of the respective simulations are marked by the free ends of the black horizontal lines away from the red curve. A horizontal line thus measures the difference between impacts on the proximal and distal compartments of a given simulated network, and whether the distal impact is smaller (horizontal line extends leftwards from curve) or greater (opposite direction) than the proximal impact. Vertical dashed lines mark the value ( on the x-axis (no impact). Upper row, niche-topology models; lower row, random-topology models; declinees in each simulation were species with the five highest niche values. Axis ranges fixed across all model configurations and intercompartment connectivities. Niche-topology models undergo greater changes in average system energy in the proximal compartment than random-topology models, es- pecially in the niche-topology models where declinees are restricted to species with the highest niche values (Fig. 6.1). The impact on average en- tropy in the proximal compartment is relatively highly positively correlated with intercompartment trophic connectivity for all model configurations and declinee niche ranges (Table 6.1). Niche-topology models tend to experience less negative and greater positive impacts in the proximal compartment as intercompartment trophic connectivity increases, especially when declinees are restricted to species with the highest niche values 132 Yangchen Lin (Fig. 6.2). Unlike niche-topology models, random-topology models have mostly negative changes in average entropy in the proximal compartment, becoming decreasingly negative with increasing intercompartment trophic connectivity (Fig. 6.2). 0 20 40 60 80 100 120 N ic h e to p o lo g y 0.0 0.5 1.0 0 20 40 60 80 100 120 0.0 0.5 1.0 0.0 0.5 1.0 0.0 0.5 1.0 0.0 0.5 1.0 0.0 0.5 1.0 R a n d o m to p o lo g y Impact on average Shannon entropy S im u la ti on s ra n ke d b y i m p ac t Intercompartment trophic connectivity levels 0.05 0.1 0.15 0.2 0.25 0.3 Figure 6.2. Pairwise comparisons between decline impacts on average entropy in proximal versus distal compartments. Detailed explanation as for Fig. 6.1. Niche-topology models undergo greater changes in community com- position in the proximal compartment than random-topology models, especially niche-topology models where the declinees are restricted to species with the highest niche values (Fig. 6.3). Impacts on the community composition of both proximal and distal compartments of niche-topology models are positively correlated with intercompartment trophic connec- tivity, while the corresponding impacts for random-topology models are negatively correlated with intercompartment trophic connectivity (Table 6.1). For all model configurations and declinee niche ranges, impacts on the power spectrum of the proximal compartment are relatively highly negatively correlated with intercompartment trophic connectivity (Table 6.1). For any given model, a large impact in the proximal compartment is usually combined with a small impact in the distal compartment, while a model experiencing a small impact in the proximal compartment may 133 Species decline in compartmentalized networks 0 20 40 60 80 100 120 N ic h e to p o lo g y 0 2 4 6 0 20 40 60 80 100 120 0 2 4 6 0 2 4 6 0 2 4 6 0 2 4 6 0 2 4 6 R a n d o m to p o lo g y Impact on community composition (RMSD) S im u la ti on s ra n ke d b y i m p ac t Intercompartment trophic connectivity levels 0.05 0.1 0.15 0.2 0.25 0.3 Figure 6.3. Pairwise comparisons between decline impacts on community composition in proximal versus distal compartments. Detailed explanation as for Fig. 6.1. sometimes experience a large impact in the distal compartment (Fig. 6.4). Compared to niche-topology models, random-topology models have a higher frequency of larger impacts on the power spectra of both proximal and distal compartments overall (Fig. 6.4). These differences between niche and random models are similar to those found in Chapter 4 (p. 76, Fig. 4.5a, extreme upper right panel). 0 20 40 60 80 100 120 N ic h e to p o lo g y 100 300 500 0 20 40 60 80 100 120 100 300 500 100 300 500 100 300 500 100 300 500 100 300 500 R a n d o m to p o lo g y Impact on power spectrum (RMSD) S im u la ti on s ra n ke d b y i m p ac t Intercompartment trophic connectivity levels 0.05 0.1 0.15 0.2 0.25 0.3 Figure 6.4. Pairwise comparisons between decline impacts on power spectra in proximal versus distal compartments. Detailed explanation as for Fig. 6.1. 134 Yangchen Lin Finally, there do not generally appear to be meaningful correlations between impacts in the proximal versus distal compartments, within each model configuration and intercompartment connectivity setting (most | | < 0:1)—the impact in the distal compartment does not seem to depend on the impact in the proximal compartment (Figs. 6.1–6.4). 6.4 discussion For all ecosystem metrics, compartmentalization has weak but systematic effects on the response of the compartments to species decline, even with the extensive parameter space of my simulations, although network topology is a more important driver. Another interesting finding is that, on average, the impact on the distal compartment usually changes little with the impact on the proximal compartment for any given model configuration and intercompartment connectivity, implying that cascading effects do not propagate far. This may seem to downplay the danger of cascading effects that have long been of concern for ecological prediction and conservation, but complacency should not be allowed to set in—the results of individual simulations (see also Appendix 6.6.2) clearly show that, depending on the specific suite of parameter values (i.e. context), the impact on the distal compartment may sometimes be relatively large even when the impact on the proximal compartment is small, suggesting a large cascade effect. Negative impacts on the average system energy of proximal com- partments of niche-topology models are greater when declinees are all top predators than when they are randomly distributed in the trophic spectrum (Appendix 6.6.2). This may be attributed to species of larger body size having higher population biomass [177], and has implications for the impact of biodiversity loss in real ecosystems, where top predators often decline first. More explicitly, lower trophic levels do not appear to increase appreciably in biomass compensating for top predator loss, meaning that a ‘topless’ ecosystem may have a diminished quantity as well as variety of ecological production, functions and socio-economically valuable services. 135 Species decline in compartmentalized networks As entropy is a measure of the effective number of species, one would expect the average entropy of the compartment in which the declines took place to change more than the other compartment. The results confirm this. Interestingly, when top predators decline in models with niche topology, the impact on entropy in the proximal compartment becomes less negative and more positive on average as intercompartment trophic connectivity increases (Fig. 6.2). This may be attributable to the randomly assigned intercompartment links ‘diluting’ the hierarchical niche topology of the compartment, evening out the energy (biomass) flows across the community. In terms of community composition, greater changes occur in niche- topology models where the declinees are top predators (Appendix 6.6.2), suggesting that top-down effects and mechanisms such as mesopredator release [394] are relatively important, at least in communities where top predators have broader-than-random diets. This again has implications for real-world scenarios where top predators are more vulnerable. The opposing behaviours of models with niche and random topologies are striking. This result underscores the importance of using appropriate network topology when modeling a given system of interest for ecological prediction and management. The slightly lower occurrence of large impacts on power spectra in either the proximal or distal compartment in models with higher intercompartment connectivity could be partly because the relatively large changes in periodicity regime caused by the perturbation in the proximal compartment are dampened or diluted more by the species in the distal compartment when the intercompartment connectivity is higher. There is considerable variation in the magnitude of impacts on power spectra across simulations, and the impacts in the proximal and distal compartments in a given simulation can be very different from each other. The higher frequency of larger impacts in random-topology models suggests that random topology tends to have lower resilience to perturbations in terms of the manner in which the community oscillates, as characterized by the power spectrum. The variability of impacts, also observed in the other 136 Yangchen Lin ecosystem metrics to a lesser extent, is a symptom of context dependency, and again underscores the difficulty of predicting how perturbations will propagate from one compartment to another. The details of species traits and interaction structure mediate the exact pattern of amplitude and frequency of oscillations. It also reminds us that caution should be exercised when extrapolating ecological conclusions and management recommendations for a specific system to other systems that, even if superficially similar, may differ in important details. There is considerable scope for future research into network com- partmentalization in ecology and beyond. Firstly, it remains to be seen whether the results would change with the number of compartments. A particularly interesting aspect of this question is that the answer may depend on how multiple compartments are connected to one another. The two-compartment case is nevertheless useful for understanding how perturbation propagates from one compartment to another, especially when two-compartment systems can occur at some spatial scale in real- ity, for example aquatic-terrestrial. Secondly, it is possible to combine compartmentalizations at different scales for more realistic representa- tions that mimic the nested or fractal structure of nature, somewhat akin to the ‘super networks’ proposed by Olesen et al. [99]. For exam- ple, Scotti et al. [100] modeled conspecific individual-individual networks within species interacting in food webs that are in turn linked to one another at the landscape level. Their model, however, lacked key inter- actions such as individual-based interspecific interactions and nontrophic interactions. Thirdly, the compartmentalized network approach could offer new insights into the emerging concept of keystone communities [395] whose removals have disproportionate dynamical impacts on metacommu- nities. The network approach can accomplish this by enabling important dynamic species interactions to be captured in ecosystem-level indicators of metacommunity resilience. The findings of this study contrast with some other findings about other kinds of networks. For example, intermediate rather than minimum or maximum connectivity between power grid networks has been found 137 Species decline in compartmentalized networks to minimize the risk and size of cascades [378]. This may be due either to real differences in the processes governing different kinds of networks, or possibly to my model assumptions such as equal average strengths of intra- and intercompartment interactions. More mechanistic studies like these will progressively give us a better idea of the degree of context dependence of the relationship between compartmentalization and network robustness to node failure. Furthermore, even deeper insights may be gained by examining compartmentalization as an effect as well as a cause, an ecological example being the effect of invasions on the way plant- pollinator networks are compartmentalized [396]. Finally, pervading the various proposed avenues for further inquiry is the challenge of acquiring data on compartmentalization in ecological networks at the large scale that has recently been efficiently accomplished [397] for compartments in other kinds of large real-world networks, such as social networks and the Internet. 6.5 conclusion My results show that ecological network compartmentalization can mediate the impacts of species decline in different ways depending on the ecosystem property of interest, implying that there are no straightforward rules governing how compartmentalization affects ecosystem stability or resilience. In addition, the impact in the proximal compartment not only exists as a direct effect of the decline, but also changes with the intercompartment connectivity. These phenomena underscore an important characteristic of the system, that of two-way feedback: perturbations in one compartment are not only propagated to the other compartment but also ‘bounced’ back to the originating compartment, sometimes as a buffering effect. The results also suggest that both network topology and declinee body size may be important in predicting the impacts of declines on compartmentalized networks. My simulation configuration of top predators declining from the niche-topology network is probably the most frequent real-world scenario among those tested, based on empirical observations of species loss [398] and food web structure (§2.1). It should 138 Yangchen Lin also be appreciated that overarching all of the above observations is the context dependence clearly manifested in my results as variation across individual model simulations—an emergent observation not apparent in isolated ecological studies of individual systems, of which only those with ‘positive’ results tend to get published. I recommend that efforts should continue to face up to the challenge of context dependence in ecology, because it has implications not only for the understanding of the complexity of ecological systems, but also for practical management. There have been calls for combining food web theory and landscape ecology to understand food webs at the landscape level [399, 400]; my work takes a step in that direction, incorporating community dynamics and different types of species interactions. 139 Species decline in compartmentalized networks 6.6 appendix 6.6.1 regression trees Regression tree for each of four ecosystem metrics, showing how vari- ous explanatory variables mediate the impact of species decline in the proximal (a) and distal (b) compartments of simulated ecological networks. Explanatory variables: topo, network topology; trocon, intercompartment trophic connectivity; mutcon, intercompartment mutualistic connectivity; decnicherange, declinee niche range; decbasal, number of basal decli- nee species; dectrolnk, number of declinee species with intercompartment trophic links; decmutlnk, number of declinee species with intercompart- ment mutualistic links; trocen, average trophic centrality of declinees; mutcen, average mutualistic centrality of declinees. Only the nodes with the greatest deviances are split, to show the most important explanatory variables. 140 Y angchen L in topo: niche decnicherange: full trocen < 9:9 decbasal < 1:5 decbasal < 3:5 decmutlnk < 9:5 −0:140 −0:200 −0:140 −0:130 −0:160 −0:130 −0:170 −0:250 −0:240 −0:230 −0:250 −0:280 −0:079 Average energya topo: niche decnicherange: full decbasal < 1:5 trocon < 0:175 dectrolnk < 5:5 decbasal < 2:5 −0:0220 0:0087 −0:0120 −0:0260 −0:0053 0:0290 0:0140 −0:0005 0:0230 0:0440 0:0280 0:0530 −0:0520 Average entropy topo: niche decnicherange: full decbasal < 1:5 trocen < 9:9 decbasal < 3:5 trocen < 11:5 0:44 0:70 0:53 0:40 0:58 0:50 0:67 0:88 0:81 1:10 0:86 1:20 0:18 Community composition topo: niche trocon < 0:075 dectrolnk < 9:5 trocen < 8:5 dectrolnk < 1:5 mutcen < 5:8 mutcen < 7:5 18 13 19 11 23 28 32 26 47 12 68 100 44 25 19 Power spectrum 141 Species decline in com partm entalized netw orks topo: niche trocen < 11:3 dectrolnk < 7:5 trocen < 13:7 −0:0190 −0:0290 −0:0200 −0:0150 −0:0250 −0:0400 −0:0370 −0:0530 −0:0089 Average energyb topo: niche trocen < 11:3 dectrolnk < 7:5 dectrolnk < 6:5 −0:0044 −0:0011 −0:0029 0:0011 −0:0032 0:0016 −0:0078 −0:0054 −0:0086 Average entropy topo: niche trocon < 0:175 trocon < 0:075 decbasal < 2:5 trocen < 14:3 0:180 0:260 0:240 0:220 0:250 0:290 0:270 0:310 0:300 0:360 0:099 Community composition topo: niche trocon < 0:075 decbasal < 2:5 trocon < 0:225 trocen < 5:9 mutcen < 8:1 15:0 11:0 17:0 21:0 13:0 9:5 19:0 21:0 51:0 79:0 24:0 21:0 16:0 Power spectrum 142 Yangchen Lin 6.6.2 plots from all treatments In each subpanel, the x-axis is the impact on the ecosystem metric and the y-axis is the rank of simulations sorted in increasing order of the impact on the ecosystem metric in the proximal compartment, demarcated by the thick curve. Impacts on the distal compartments of the respective simulations are marked by the free ends of the grey horizontal lines away from the curve. A horizontal line thus measures the difference between impacts on the proximal and distal compartments of a given simulated network, and whether the distal impact is smaller (horizontal line extends leftwards from curve) or greater (opposite direction) than the proximal impact. Vertical dashed lines mark the value 0 on the x-axis (no impact). Axis ranges fixed across all model configurations and intercompartment connectivities within each ecosystem metric. 143 Species decline in compartmentalized networks −0.6 −0.2 0.2 0 20 40 60 80 100 120 Impact on average system energy S im u la ti on s ra n ke d b y i m p ac t Intercompartment trophic connectivity levels In te rc om p ar tm en t m u tu al is ti c co n n ec ti v it y l ev el s 0.05 0. 05 0.1 0. 1 0.15 0. 15 0.2 0. 2 0.25 0. 25 0.3 0. 3 Average system energy: declinees from full niche range, niche topology. 144 Yangchen Lin −0.6 −0.2 0.2 0 20 40 60 80 100 120 Impact on average system energy S im u la ti on s ra n ke d b y i m p ac t Intercompartment trophic connectivity levels In te rc om p ar tm en t m u tu al is ti c co n n ec ti v it y l ev el s 0.05 0. 05 0.1 0. 1 0.15 0. 15 0.2 0. 2 0.25 0. 25 0.3 0. 3 Average system energy: declinees from full niche range, random topology. 145 Species decline in compartmentalized networks −0.6 −0.2 0.2 0 20 40 60 80 100 120 Impact on average system energy S im u la ti on s ra n ke d b y i m p ac t Intercompartment trophic connectivity levels In te rc om p ar tm en t m u tu al is ti c co n n ec ti v it y l ev el s 0.05 0. 05 0.1 0. 1 0.15 0. 15 0.2 0. 2 0.25 0. 25 0.3 0. 3 Average system energy: declinees from high niche range, niche topology. 146 Yangchen Lin −0.6 −0.2 0.2 0 20 40 60 80 100 120 Impact on average system energy S im u la ti on s ra n ke d b y i m p ac t Intercompartment trophic connectivity levels In te rc om p ar tm en t m u tu al is ti c co n n ec ti v it y l ev el s 0.05 0. 05 0.1 0. 1 0.15 0. 15 0.2 0. 2 0.25 0. 25 0.3 0. 3 Average system energy: declinees from high niche range, random topology. 147 Species decline in compartmentalized networks −0.5 0.0 0.5 1.0 0 20 40 60 80 100 120 Impact on average Shannon entropy S im u la ti on s ra n ke d b y i m p ac t Intercompartment trophic connectivity levels In te rc om p ar tm en t m u tu al is ti c co n n ec ti v it y l ev el s 0.05 0. 05 0.1 0. 1 0.15 0. 15 0.2 0. 2 0.25 0. 25 0.3 0. 3 Average entropy: declinees from full niche range, niche topology. 148 Yangchen Lin −0.5 0.0 0.5 1.0 0 20 40 60 80 100 120 Impact on average Shannon entropy S im u la ti on s ra n ke d b y i m p ac t Intercompartment trophic connectivity levels In te rc om p ar tm en t m u tu al is ti c co n n ec ti v it y l ev el s 0.05 0. 05 0.1 0. 1 0.15 0. 15 0.2 0. 2 0.25 0. 25 0.3 0. 3 Average entropy: declinees from full niche range, random topology. 149 Species decline in compartmentalized networks −0.5 0.0 0.5 1.0 0 20 40 60 80 100 120 Impact on average Shannon entropy S im u la ti on s ra n ke d b y i m p ac t Intercompartment trophic connectivity levels In te rc om p ar tm en t m u tu al is ti c co n n ec ti v it y l ev el s 0.05 0. 05 0.1 0. 1 0.15 0. 15 0.2 0. 2 0.25 0. 25 0.3 0. 3 Average entropy: declinees from high niche range, niche topology. 150 Yangchen Lin −0.5 0.0 0.5 1.0 0 20 40 60 80 100 120 Impact on average Shannon entropy S im u la ti on s ra n ke d b y i m p ac t Intercompartment trophic connectivity levels In te rc om p ar tm en t m u tu al is ti c co n n ec ti v it y l ev el s 0.05 0. 05 0.1 0. 1 0.15 0. 15 0.2 0. 2 0.25 0. 25 0.3 0. 3 Average entropy: declinees from high niche range, random topology. 151 Species decline in compartmentalized networks 0 2 4 6 0 20 40 60 80 100 120 Impact on community composition (RMSD) S im u la ti on s ra n ke d b y i m p ac t Intercompartment trophic connectivity levels In te rc om p ar tm en t m u tu al is ti c co n n ec ti v it y l ev el s 0.05 0. 05 0.1 0. 1 0.15 0. 15 0.2 0. 2 0.25 0. 25 0.3 0. 3 Community composition: declinees from full niche range, niche topology. 152 Yangchen Lin 0 2 4 6 0 20 40 60 80 100 120 Impact on community composition (RMSD) S im u la ti on s ra n ke d b y i m p ac t Intercompartment trophic connectivity levels In te rc om p ar tm en t m u tu al is ti c co n n ec ti v it y l ev el s 0.05 0. 05 0.1 0. 1 0.15 0. 15 0.2 0. 2 0.25 0. 25 0.3 0. 3 Community composition: declinees from full niche range, random topology. 153 Species decline in compartmentalized networks 0 2 4 6 0 20 40 60 80 100 120 Impact on community composition (RMSD) S im u la ti on s ra n ke d b y i m p ac t Intercompartment trophic connectivity levels In te rc om p ar tm en t m u tu al is ti c co n n ec ti v it y l ev el s 0.05 0. 05 0.1 0. 1 0.15 0. 15 0.2 0. 2 0.25 0. 25 0.3 0. 3 Community composition: declinees from high niche range, niche topology. 154 Yangchen Lin 0 2 4 6 0 20 40 60 80 100 120 Impact on community composition (RMSD) S im u la ti on s ra n ke d b y i m p ac t Intercompartment trophic connectivity levels In te rc om p ar tm en t m u tu al is ti c co n n ec ti v it y l ev el s 0.05 0. 05 0.1 0. 1 0.15 0. 15 0.2 0. 2 0.25 0. 25 0.3 0. 3 Community composition: declinees from high niche range, random topology. 155 Species decline in compartmentalized networks 0 200 400 600 0 20 40 60 80 100 120 Impact on power spectrum (RMSD) S im u la ti on s ra n ke d b y i m p ac t Intercompartment trophic connectivity levels In te rc om p ar tm en t m u tu al is ti c co n n ec ti v it y l ev el s 0.05 0. 05 0.1 0. 1 0.15 0. 15 0.2 0. 2 0.25 0. 25 0.3 0. 3 Power spectrum: declinees from full niche range, niche topology. 156 Yangchen Lin 0 200 400 600 0 20 40 60 80 100 120 Impact on power spectrum (RMSD) S im u la ti on s ra n ke d b y i m p ac t Intercompartment trophic connectivity levels In te rc om p ar tm en t m u tu al is ti c co n n ec ti v it y l ev el s 0.05 0. 05 0.1 0. 1 0.15 0. 15 0.2 0. 2 0.25 0. 25 0.3 0. 3 Power spectrum: declinees from full niche range, random topology. 157 Species decline in compartmentalized networks 0 200 400 600 0 20 40 60 80 100 120 Impact on power spectrum (RMSD) S im u la ti on s ra n ke d b y i m p ac t Intercompartment trophic connectivity levels In te rc om p ar tm en t m u tu al is ti c co n n ec ti v it y l ev el s 0.05 0. 05 0.1 0. 1 0.15 0. 15 0.2 0. 2 0.25 0. 25 0.3 0. 3 Power spectrum: declinees from high niche range, niche topology. 158 Yangchen Lin 0 200 400 600 0 20 40 60 80 100 120 Impact on power spectrum (RMSD) S im u la ti on s ra n ke d b y i m p ac t Intercompartment trophic connectivity levels In te rc om p ar tm en t m u tu al is ti c co n n ec ti v it y l ev el s 0.05 0. 05 0.1 0. 1 0.15 0. 15 0.2 0. 2 0.25 0. 25 0.3 0. 3 Power spectrum: declinees from high niche range, random topology. 159 The author's sanctum for inner contemplation. Mediæval brick arch (1490) and timber columns (1912) beneath the oriel window of the Long Gallery, Queens' College, Cambridge. Photograph by the author exhibited at the Arts Festival (2014) in the Old Hall (1449, visible at upper left), Queens' College. 7 coda As ecologists, we are very good at finding linear relationships while probing one component of a specific system under a specific set of con- ditions, but when it comes to the totality of interdependent components under diverse conditions, we hitherto have only a very blurry image. This thesis, synthesizing a broad range of fundamental mechanisms of species interaction in ecological network dynamics, has tried taking some tentative steps towards beginning to clarify that image. There are two overall revelations: that the structure, dynamics and perturbation of com- plex ecological networks have effects that are highly context-dependent, and that various interesting systematic patterns nevertheless emerge above this context dependence. Where do we go next? One obvious path is towards application to real-world conservation. A systems approach can facilitate preventive proactive conservation, because one is less likely to let problems develop unnoticed in one part of the system while preoccupied with conserving another part. The public may be reluctant to implement system-level conservation measures, however, if such measures do not have direct and obvious benefits to their immediate component (e.g. charismatic species) of interest [19, 116]. There is also the challenge of identifying metrics can be easily measured in the field that are indicative of ecosystem health [401]. Nevertheless, we are increasingly conscious of the need for ‘system-level conservation ecology’ [38] and ‘conservation of networks’ [402], although some calls for exploiting the power of systems approaches in conservation still focus on particular species as the final deliverable [403]. The pioneering steps have been taken, such as in a field study of the consequences of urbanization on the structure of bird-plant networks [404], helping to understand demographic impacts of environmental change. In time, scientific advancements and changes in public perception will hopefully enable the full potential of systems strategies to be realized. Meanwhile, given that theoretical studies constitute a sizeable proportion of systems ecology research owing to data scarcity, it might be worthwhile considering whether and how 163 Coda theoretical modeling outputs could serve as evidence for the evidence- based conservation paradigm championed by Sutherland et al. [405]. More specific to modeling methodology, some overarching issues remain to be addressed in future work. It would be interesting to investigate network topologies that are influenced more by phylogeny, or other drivers, than by body size; research in this direction has so far considered different interaction types in isolation [24, 406–409]. Yan & Zhang [410] also looked at how interactions that do not increase monotonically with population density affect community persistence, although they did it separately for predator-prey and bipartite mutualistic dynamic networks under a deterministic framework and did not subject their model communities to exogenous perturbation; there is scope here for more realistic, yet not drastically more complex, models of the type described in my work. There is also a recent rise of an eco-evolutionary perspective on ecological networks, ranging from the relationships between species’ taxonomies and their topological positions and functions in food webs [285], to eco- evolutionary dynamics and their implications for climate change [411], and to using networks of interacting computer programmes to mimic and understand species evolution in ecological networks [412]. Such research has exciting prospects for not only ecology but also complexity science at large, as it may extend to more general cases of broad scientific, cultural and policy interest, such as the evolution of technology. Overall, a strategy of using a hierarchy of models of varying levels of complexity to more comprehensively understand the various aspects of the system [413] may be employed to explore all these avenues. Zooming out again to see both the details and the bigger picture at the same time, one might envision a synthesis of general laws allying ecological networks with complex networks in other disciplines (see §1.2). Knowledge gleaned from the analysis of big data in other kinds of networks could provide insights into properties of ecological networks for which data are scarce. Conversely, ecological networks could potentially provide valuable insights into combinatorial optimization problems in manmade networks, analogous to the way that the adaptive spatial network evolution of 164 Yangchen Lin slime molds has been found to help simultaneously optimize transport networks for minimal cost, maximal efficiency and maximal fault tolerance [414]. Such insights could even apply to the arts and entertainment. For example, I would say that motion pictures with complex feedbacks between multiple players resulting in unexpected nonlinear evolution of the story line may be more exciting. Going back to ecology, an interesting and useful research and man- agement strategy is to look at ecological systems actually interacting with other systems. In modeling terms, this could take the form of an ecological network ‘plug-in’ to system dynamics models of the wider environment. Indeed, linking ecological models with economic, social and other kinds of models has been suggested as a way to make ecology more useful to policy makers and practitioners [415]. Various researchers have pioneered the so-called social-ecological or bio-economic models, with conceptual models (see Milner-Gulland [416] for an overview) and simulation models [417, 418] synthesizing ecological system dynamics, hu- man behaviour and management policies. Complex systems science has also been applied to the management of actual social-ecological systems [419, 420]. These efforts, however, have not yet embraced the interaction complexities of ecological networks. In the policy arena, this multidisciplinary system dynamics approach could prove useful for further boosting the effectiveness of horizon scanning, with the aim of identifying issues of potentially far-reaching future consequences for the global environment [421, 422]. The value of horizon scanning lies not only in identifying a specific issue but also in being able to infer its possible interactions with other components of the system that could engender unexpected and disproportionate consequences for seemingly unrelated components and for the system as a whole. System dynamics and complexity science at large are poised to tackle this challenge at a point in history that many have hailed as the ‘Information Revolution’ (drawing inspiration from the Industrial Revolution). Related fields such as risk analysis [330, 331], and info-gap decision theory [423] where the lack of data and complexity of processes 165 Coda make probability calculations impossible, also stand to gain from the insights of complexity science. Such advancements in networks and complex systems are setting the stage for us to be more proactive with having an oversight and control of everything to secure the well-being of mankind [366, 424]; recall the discussion on network control (§4.4). But caution is needed here, for in trying to suppress unfavourable but relatively small ‘earthquakes’ in complex networks, we may inadvertently force a buildup of pressure that triggers big catastrophes, although conversely we might judiciously encourage small cascades in order to avoid a big one. Rigorous control measures may also have the side effect of making the world more tedious, more regimented and less exciting to live in, as well as possibly reducing its adaptability to unexpected exogenous drivers. We should use our understanding of complexity to exist harmoniously within nature, acknowledging the fact that things cannot always be in our favour if overall resilience is to be sustained. An ecosystem does not try to maximize one overall goal, because its constituent agents engage in conflicting selective processes [425]; this, I argue, is what gives rise to diversity and resilience in the face of change. We are seeing something unprecedented in the history of life on earth—an especially intelligent species actually having global oversight rather than solely abiding by local rules giving rise to self-organisation. Who knows where this will lead us? 166 literature cited [1] Lin, Y. & Sutherland, W. J. 2013. Color and degree of interspecific synchrony of environmental noise affect the variability of complex ecological networks. Ecol. Model. 263:162–173. [2] 2014. Extinction and invasion do not add up in noisy dynamic ecological networks. Basic Appl. Ecol. 15:475–485. [3] 2014. Interaction modification effects on ecological networks are affected by ratio dependence and network topology. J. Theor. Biol. 363:151–157. [4] 2015. Compartmentalization influences the response of bioenergetic ecological networks to species declines. J. Complex Netw. doi:10.1093/comnet/cnv007. [5] Assaneo, F. et al. 2013. Dynamics and coexistence in a system with intraguild mutualism. Ecol. Complex. 14:64–74. [6] Peh, K. S.-H. et al. 2014. Forest fragmentation and ecosystem function. In: Kettle, C. & Koh, L. P., eds. Global Forest Fragmentation. cabi, United Kingdom pp. 96–114. [7] Kauffman, S. 2000. Investigations. Oxford University Press, New York. [8] Prigogine, I. 1997. The End of Certainty: Time, Chaos, and the New Laws of Nature. The Free Press, New York. [9] Kareiva, P. 1994. Higher order interactions as a foil to reductionist ecology. Ecology 75(6):1527–1528. [10] Low-Décarie, E. et al. 2014. Rising complexity and falling explanatory power in ecology. Front. Ecol. Environ. 12(7):412–418. [11] Evans, M.R. et al. 2013. Predictive systems ecology. Proc. R. Soc. B 280:20131452. [12] Strogatz, S.H. 2001. Exploring complex networks. Nature 410:268–276. [13] Barabási, A.-L. 2012. The network takeover. Nat. Phys. 8:14–16. [14] Schich, M. et al. 2014. A network framework of cultural history. Science 345:558–562. [15] Solé, R. V. & Valverde, S. 2004. Information theory of complex networks: on evolu- tion and architectural constraints. Lect. Notes Phys. 650:189–207. [16] Proulx, S. R. et al. 2005. Network thinking in ecology and evolution. Trends Ecol. Evol. 20(6):345–353. [17] Bascompte, J. 2009. Disentangling the web of life. Science 325:416–419. [18] Brose, U. 2010. Improving nature conservancy strategies by ecological network theory. Basic Appl. Ecol. 11:1–5. [19] Lewinsohn, T.M. & Cagnolo, L. 2012. Keystones in a tangled bank. Science 335:1449– 1451. [20] Polis, G. A. 1991. Complex trophic interactions in deserts: an empirical critique of food-web theory. Am. Nat. 138(1):123–155. 167 Literature cited [21] Cardinale, B. et al. 2009. Towards a food web perspective on biodiversity and ecosys- tem functioning. In: Naeem, S. et al., eds. Biodiversity, Ecosystem Functioning, & Human Wellbeing. Oxford University Press pp. 105–120. [22] Duffy, J. E. et al. 2007. The functional role of biodiversity in ecosystems: incorporat- ing trophic complexity. Ecol. Lett. 10:522–538. [23] Loreau, M. & Thébault, E. 2005. Food webs and the relationship between biodiver- sity and ecosystem functioning. In: de Ruiter, P. C. et al., eds. Dynamic Food Webs: Multispecies Assemblages, Ecosystem Development and Environmental Change. Academic Press, Massachusetts pp. 270–282. [24] Chiu, G. S. & Westveld, A.H. 2011. A unifying approach for food webs, phylogeny, social networks, and statistics. P. Natl. Acad. Sci. usa 108(38):15881–15886. [25] Brummitt, C.D. et al. 2013. Transdisciplinary electric power grid science. P. Natl. Acad. Sci. usa 110(30):12159. [26] Haldane, A.G. & May, R.M. 2011. Systemic risk in banking ecosystems. Nature 469:351–355. [27] Arinaminpathy, N. et al. 2012. Size and complexity in model financial systems. P. Natl. Acad. Sci. usa 109(45):18338–18343. [28] Shore, J. et al. 2013. Power laws and fragility in flow networks. Social Networks 35:116– 123. [29] DasGupta, B. & Kaligounder, L. 2014. On global stability of financial networks. J. Complex Netw. 2:313–354. [30] Aufderheide, H. et al. 2013. How to predict community responses to perturbations in the face of imperfect knowledge and network complexity. Proc. R. Soc. B 280:20132355. [31] Rombouts, I. et al. 2013. Food web indicators under the Marine Strategy Framework Directive: from complexity to simplicity? Ecol. Indic. 29:246–254. [32] Wells, K. et al. 2014. Population fluctuations affect inference in ecological networks of multi-species interactions. Oikos 123:589–598. [33] Holme, P. & Saramäki, J. 2012. Temporal networks. Physics Reports 519:97–125. [34] Vespignani, A. 2012. Modelling dynamical processes in complex socio-technical sys- tems. Nat. Phys. 8:32–39. [35] Barzel, B. & Barabási, A.-L. 2013. Universality in network dynamics. Nat. Phys. 9:673– 681. [36] Bassett, D. S. et al. 2013. Robust detection of dynamic community structure in net- works. Chaos 23:013142. [37] Suweis, S. & D’Odorico, P. 2014. Early warning signs in social-ecological networks. PLoS ONE 9(7):e101851. [38] Bascompte, J. & Stouffer, D. B. 2009. The assembly and disassembly of ecological networks. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 364:1781–1787. 168 Literature cited [39] Eveleigh, E. S. et al. 2007. Fluctuations in density of an outbreak species drive diver- sity cascades in food webs. P. Natl. Acad. Sci. usa 104(43):16976–16981. [40] Golubski, A. J. & Abrams, P. A. 2011. Modifying modifiers: what happens when inter- specific interactions interact? J. Anim. Ecol. 80:1097–1108. [41] Kéfi, S. et al. 2012. More than a meal … integrating non-feeding interactions into food webs. Ecol. Lett. 15:291–300. [42] Melián, C. J. et al. 2009. Diversity in a complex ecological network with two interac- tion types. Oikos 118:122–130. [43] Blonder, B. et al. 2012. Temporal dynamics and network analysis. Methods Ecol. Evol. 3:958–972. [44] Lawton, J.H. 1999. Are there general laws in ecology? Oikos 84:177–192. [45] Scheffer, M. & Carpenter, S. R. 2003. Catastrophic regime shifts in ecosystems: linking theory to observation. Trends Ecol. Evol. 18(12):648–656. [46] Pimm, S. L. & Lawton, J.H. 1977. Number of trophic levels in ecological communities. Nature 268:329–331. [47] 1978. On feeding on more than one trophic level. Nature 275:542–544. [48] Pimm, S. L. 1979. The structure of food webs. Theor. Pop. Biol. 16:144–158. [49] Yodzis, P. 1981. The stability of real ecosystems. Nature 289:674–676. [50] Dambacher, J.M. et al. 2003. Qualitative predictions in model ecosystems. Ecol. Model. 161:79–93. [51] Allesina, S. & Tang, S. 2012. Stability criteria for complex ecosystems. Nature 483:205– 208. [52] Melbourne-Thomas, J. et al. 2012. Comprehensive evaluation of model uncertainty in qualitative network analyses. Ecol. Monogr. 82(4):505–519. [53] Gross, T. et al. 2009. Generalized models reveal stabilizing factors in food webs. Science 325:747–750. [54] Yeakel, J. D. et al. 2011. Generalized modeling of ecological population dynamics. Theor. Ecol. 4:179–194. [55] Berlow, E. L. et al. 2004. Interaction strengths in food webs: issues and opportunities. J. Anim. Ecol. 73:585–598. [56] Janson, S. 2011. Networking—smoothly does it. Science 333:298–299. [57] Rohr, R. P. et al. 2014. On the structural stability of mutualistic systems. Science 345:1253497. [58] McCann, K. S. 2000. The diversity-stability debate. Nature 405:228–233. [59] Rohde, K. 2005. Nonequilibrium Ecology. Cambridge University Press. 169 Literature cited [60] Evans, M.R. et al. 2013. Do simple models lead to generality in ecology? Trends Ecol. Evol. 28(10):578–583. [61] Amano, T. 2012. Unravelling the dynamics of organisms in a changing world using ecological modelling. Ecol. Res. 27:495–507. [62] Evans, M.R. 2012. Modelling ecological systems in a changing world. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 367:181–190. [63] Evans, M.R. et al. 2014. Data availability and model complexity, generality, and utility: a reply to Lonergan. Trends Ecol. Evol. 29(6):302–303. [64] Wollrab, S. et al. 2012. Simple rules describe bottom-up and top-down control in food webs with alternative energy pathways. Ecol. Lett. 15:935–946. [65] Condie, S. A. et al. 2014. Relating food web structure to resilience, keystone status and uncertainty in ecological responses. Ecosphere 5(7):81. [66] Lang, B. et al. 2014. Effects of environmental warming and drought on size-structured soil food webs. Oikos 123:1224–1233. [67] Brose, U. et al. 2005. Scaling up keystone effects from simple to complex ecological networks. Ecol. Lett. 8:1317–1325. [68] May, R.M. 1972. Will a large complex system be stable? Nature 238:413–414. [69] 1974. Stability and Complexity in Model Ecosystems 2nd ed. Monographs in Popu- lation Biology 6. Princeton University Press, New Jersey. [70] McNaughton, S. J. 1978. Stability and diversity of ecological communities. Nature 274:251–253. [71] Pimm, S. L. 1984. The complexity and stability of ecosystems. Nature 307:321–326. [72] Polis, G. A. 1998. Stability is woven by complex webs. Nature 395:744–745. [73] Rudolf, V.H.W. & Lafferty, K.D. 2011. Stage structure alters how complexity affects stability of ecological networks. Ecol. Lett. 14:75–79. [74] Rooney, N. & McCann, K. S. 2012. Integrating food web diversity, structure and stability. Trends Ecol. Evol. 27(1):40–46. [75] Neutel, A.-M. & Thorne, M.A. S. 2014. Interaction strengths in balanced carbon cycles and the absence of a relation between ecosystem complexity and stability. Ecol. Lett. 17:651–661. [76] Lewis, O.T. 2009. Biodiversity change and ecosystem function in tropical forests. Basic Appl. Ecol. 10:97–102. [77] Mason, P.H. 2014. Degeneracy: demystifying and destigmatizing a core concept in systems biology. Complexity 20(3):12–21. [78] Coreau, A. et al. 2010. Exploring the difficulties of studying futures in ecology: what do ecological scientists think? Oikos 119:1364–1376. [79] Patten, B. C. 1992. Energy, emergy and environs. Ecol. Model. 62:29–69. 170 Literature cited [80] Ulanowicz, R. E. 2004. Quantitative methods for ecological network analysis. Comput. Biol. Chem. 28:321–339. [81] Fath, B.D. et al. 2007. Ecological network analysis: network construction. Ecol. Model. 208:49–55. [82] Olff, H. et al. 2009. Parallel ecological networks in ecosystems. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 364:1755–1779. [83] Raffaelli, D. et al. 2005. Do marine and terrestrial ecologists do it differently? Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 304:283–289. [84] Vasas, V. & Jordán, F. 2006. Topological keystone species in ecological interaction networks: considering link quality and non-trophic effects. Ecol. Model. 196:365–378. [85] Dame, R. F. & Patten, B. C. 1981. Analysis of energy flows in an intertidal oyster reef. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 5:115–124. [86] Baird, D. & Ulanowicz, R. E. 1989. The seasonal dynamics of the Chesapeake Bay ecosystem. Ecol. Monogr. 59(4):329–364. [87] Cross, W. F. et al. 2013. Food-web dynamics in a large river discontinuum. Ecol. Monogr. 83(3):311–337. [88] Hoover, C. et al. 2013. Effects of hunting, fishing and climate change on the Hudson Bay marine ecosystem: II. Ecosystem model future projections. Ecol. Model. 264:143– 156. [89] Schaubroeck, T. et al. 2012. Improved ecological network analysis for environmental sustainability assessment: a case study on a forest ecosystem. Ecol. Model. 247:144–156. [90] Libralato, S. et al. 2010. Food-web traits of protected and exploited areas of the Adriatic Sea. Biol. Conserv. 143:2182–2194. [91] Fath, B.D. & Patten, B. C. 1998. Network synergism: emergence of positive relations in ecological systems. Ecol. Model. 107:127–143. [92] Fath, B.D. 2007. Network mutualism: positive community-level relations in ecosys- tems. Ecol. Model. 208:56–67. [93] Yodzis, P. & Innes, S. 1992. Body size and consumer-resource dynamics. Am. Nat. 139(6):1151–1175. [94] Williams, R. J. & Martinez, N.D. 2004. Stabilization of chaotic and non-permanent food-web dynamics. Eur. Phys. J. B 38:297–303. [95] Brose, U. et al. 2006. Allometric scaling enhances stability in complex food webs. Ecol. Lett. 9:1228–1236. [96] Williams, R. J. & Martinez, N.D. 2008. Success and its limits among structural models of complex food webs. J. Anim. Ecol. 77:512–519. [97] Gilbert, B. et al. 2014. A bioenergetic framework for the temperature dependence of trophic interactions. Ecol. Lett. 17:902–914. 171 Literature cited [98] Garay-Narváez, L. et al. 2014. Food web modularity and biodiversity promote species persistence in polluted environments. Oikos 123:583–588. [99] Olesen, J.M. et al. 2010. From Broadstone to Zackenberg: space, time and hierarchies in ecological networks. Adv. Ecol. Res. 42:1–69. [100] Scotti, M. et al. 2013. Social and landscape effects on food webs: a multi-level network simulation model. J. Complex Netw. 1:160–182. [101] Carmichael, T. & Hadzikadic, M. 2013. Emergent features in a general food web simulation: Lotka-Volterra, Gause’s law, and the paradox of enrichment. Advances in Complex Systems 16(8):1350014. [102] Dyer, L. A. et al. 2010. Diversity of interactions: a metric for studies of biodiversity. Biotropica 42(3):281–289. [103] Novotny, V. et al. 2010. Guild-specific patterns of species richness and host special- ization in plant-herbivore food webs from a tropical forest. J. Anim. Ecol. 79:1193– 1203. [104] Yeakel, J. D. et al. 2014. Collapse of an ecological network in Ancient Egypt. P. Natl. Acad. Sci. usa 111(40):14472–14477. [105] Lafferty, K.D. et al. 2008. Parasites in food webs: the ultimate missing links. Ecol. Lett. 11:533–546. [106] Sukhdeo, M. V.K. 2010. Food webs for parasitologists: a review. J. Parasitol. 96(2):273– 284. [107] Preston, D.L. et al. 2012. Food web including infectious agents for a California freshwater pond. Ecology 93(7):1760. [108] Reagan, D. P. & Waide, R. B., eds. 1996. The Food Web of a Tropical Rain Forest. Uni- versity of Chicago Press, Chicago. [109] Bascompte, J. et al. 2005. Interaction strength combinations and the overfishing of a marine food web. P. Natl. Acad. Sci. usa 102(15):5443–5447. [110] Jacob, U. et al. 2011. The role of body size in complex food webs: a cold case. Adv. Ecol. Res. 45:181–223. [111] Planque, B. et al. 2014. Who eats whom in the Barents Sea: a food web topology from plankton to whales. Ecology 95(5):1430. [112] Wirta, H.K. et al. 2014. Complementary molecular information changes our percep- tion of food web structure. P. Natl. Acad. Sci. usa 111(5):1885–1890. [113] Vinagre, C. & Costa, M. J. 2014. Estuarine-coastal gradient in food web network structure and properties. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 503:11–21. [114] Schmitz, O. 2010. Resolving Ecosystem Complexity. Monographs in Population Biology 47. Princeton University Press, New Jersey. [115] Hampton, S. E. et al. 2013. Big data and the future of ecology. Front. Ecol. Environ. 11(3):156–162. 172 Literature cited [116] Pocock, M. J.O. et al. 2012. The robustness and restoration of a network of ecological networks. Science 335:973–977. [117] Kéfi, S. et al. 2015. Network structure beyond food webs: mapping non-trophic and trophic interactions on Chilean rocky shores. Ecology 96(1):291–303. [118] Novak, M. et al. 2011. Predicting community responses to perturbations in the face of imperfect knowledge and network complexity. Ecology 92(4):836–846. [119] Yodzis, P. 1988. The indeterminacy of ecological interactions as perceived through perturbation experiments. Ecology 69(2):508–515. [120] Kuwae, T. et al. 2012. Variable and complex food web structures revealed by exploring missing trophic links between birds and biofilm. Ecol. Lett. 15:347–356. [121] Boit, A. et al. 2012. Mechanistic theory and modelling of complex food-web dynamics in Lake Constance. Ecol. Lett. 15:594–602. [122] Hudson, L.N. & Reuman, D.C. 2013. A cure for the plague of parameters: con- straining models of complex population dynamics with allometries. Proc. R. Soc. B 280:20131901. [123] Lonergan, M. 2014. Data availability constrains model complexity, generality, and utility: a response to Evans et al. Trends Ecol. Evol. 29(6):301–302. [124] Gardner, T. A. et al. 2007. Predicting the uncertain future of tropical forest species in a data vacuum. Biotropica 39(1):25–30. [125] Berlow, E. L. et al. 2009. Simple prediction of interaction strengths in complex food webs. P. Natl. Acad. Sci. usa 106(1):187–191. [126] Romanuk, T.N. et al. 2009. Predicting invasion success in complex ecological networks. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 364:1743–1754. [127] Macilwain, C. 2014. A touch of the random. Science 344:1221–1223. [128] Boyd, I. L. 2012. The art of ecological modeling. Science 337:306–307. [129] Carpenter, S. R. 2002. Ecological futures: building an ecology of the long now. Ecology 83(8):2069–2083. [130] Scheffer, M. 2009. Critical Transitions in Nature and Society. Princeton Studies in Com- plexity. Princeton University Press, New Jersey. [131] Purves, D. et al. 2013. Time to model all life on earth. Nature 493:295–297. [132] Ings, T. C. et al. 2009. Ecological networks—beyond food webs. J. Anim. Ecol. 78:253– 269. [133] Sterman, J.D. 2002. All models are wrong: reflections on becoming a systems scientist. Syst. Dyn. Rev. 18(4):501–531. [134] Patten, B. C. 2013. Systems ecology and environmentalism: getting the science right. Ecol. Eng. 61:446–455. 173 Literature cited [135] Patten, B. C. 2014. Systems ecology and environmentalism: getting the science right. Part I: facets for a more holistic Nature Book of ecology. Ecol. Model. 293:4–21. [136] Grimm, V. & Railsback, S. F. 2012. Pattern-oriented modelling: a ‘multi-scope’ for predictive systems ecology. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 367:298–310. [137] RCore Team 2013. R: a Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. [138] Williams, R. J. & Martinez, N.D. 2000. Simple rules yield complex food webs. Nature 404:180–183. [139] Allesina, S. et al. 2008. A general model for food web structure. Science 320:658–661. [140] Williams, R. J. 2008. Effects of network and dynamical model structure on species persistence in large model food webs. Theor. Ecol. 1:141–151. [141] Stouffer, D. B. 2010. Scaling from individuals to networks in food webs. Funct. Ecol. 24:44–51. [142] Williams, R. J. et al. 2010. The probabilistic niche model reveals the niche structure and role of body size in a complex food web. PLoS ONE 5(8):e12092. [143] Milo, R. et al. 2002. Network motifs: simple building blocks of complex networks. Science 298:824–827. [144] Bascompte, J. & Melián, C. J. 2005. Simple trophic modules for complex food webs. Ecology 86(11):2868–2873. [145] Stouffer, D. B. et al. 2007. Evidence for the existence of a robust pattern of prey selection in food webs. Proc. R. Soc. B 274:1931–1940. [146] Stouffer, D. B. et al. 2006. A robust measure of food web intervality. P. Natl. Acad. Sci. usa 103(50):19015–19020. [147] Halnes, G. et al. 2007. The modified niche model: including detritus in simple struc- tural food web models. Ecol. Model. 208:9–16. [148] Warren, C. P. et al. 2010. The inverse niche model for food webs with parasites. Theor. Ecol. 3:285–294. [149] Williams, R. J. & Purves, D. 2011. The probabilistic niche model reveals substantial variation in the niche structure of empirical food webs. Ecology 92(9):1849–1857. [150] Petchey, O.L. et al. 2011. Fit, efficiency, and biology: some thoughts on judging food web models. J. Theor. Biol. 279:169–171. [151] Pierce, G. J. & Ollason, J.G. 1987. Eight reasons why optimal foraging theory is a complete waste of time. Oikos 49(1):111–118. [152] Carmel, Y. & Ben-Haim, Y. 2005. Info-gap robust-satisficing model of foraging behavior: do foragers optimize or satisfice? Am. Nat. 166(5):633–641. [153] Rossberg, A.G. et al. 2010. Food-web structure in low- and high-dimensional trophic niche spaces. J. R. Soc. Interface 7:1735–1743. 174 Literature cited [154] Lafferty, K.D. et al. 2006. Parasites dominate food web links. P. Natl. Acad. Sci. usa 103(30):11211–11216. [155] Thieltges, D.W. et al. 2013. Parasites as prey in aquatic food webs: implications for predator infection and parasite transmission. Oikos 122:1473–1482. [156] Selakovic, S. et al. 2014. Infectious disease agents mediate interaction in food webs and ecosystems. Proc. R. Soc. B 281:20132709. [157] Martinez, N.D. 1992. Constant connectance in community food webs. Am. Nat. 139(6):1208–1218. [158] Dunne, J. A. et al. 2002. Network structure and biodiversity loss in food webs: robust- ness increases with connectance. Ecol. Lett. 5:558–567. [159] Rojas-Echenique, J. & Allesina, S. 2011. Interaction rules affect species coexistence in intransitive networks. Ecology 92(5):1174–1180. [160] Charnov, E. L. et al. 1976. Ecological implications of resource depression. Am. Nat. 110(972):247–259. [161] Gross, K. 2008. Positive interactions among competitors can produce species-rich communities. Ecol. Lett. 11:929–936. [162] Crowley, P.H. & Cox, J. J. 2011. Intraguild mutualism. Trends Ecol. Evol. 26(12):627– 633. [163] Roopin, M. et al. 2008. Nutrient transfer in a marine mutualism: patterns of ammonia excretion by anemonefish and uptake by giant sea anemones. Mar. Biol. 154:547–556. [164] Rossignol, P. A. et al. 1985. Enhanced mosquito blood-finding success on parasitemic hosts: evidence for vector-parasite mutualism. P. Natl. Acad. Sci. usa 82:7725–7727. [165] Donatti, C. I. et al. 2011. Analysis of a hyper-diverse seed dispersal network: modu- larity and underlying mechanisms. Ecol. Lett. 14:773–781. [166] Pires, M.M. et al. 2011. Do food web models reproduce the structure of mutualistic networks? PLoS ONE 6(11):e27280. [167] Suweis, S. et al. 2013. Emergence of structural and dynamical properties of ecological mutualistic networks. Nature 500:449–452. [168] Holbrook, S. J. et al. 2008. Effects of sheltering fish on growth of their host corals. Mar. Biol. 155:521–530. [169] James, A. et al. 2012. Disentangling nestedness from models of Ecol. Complex. Nature 487:227–230. [170] Jordano, P. 1987. Patterns of mutualistic interactions in pollination and seed dispersal: connectance, dependence asymmetries, and coevolution. Am. Nat. 129(5):657–677. [171] Burkholder, P. R. 1952. Cooperation and conflict among primitive organisms. Am. Sci. 40(4):601–631. [172] Newman, M.E. J. 2010. Networks: an Introduction. Oxford University Press. 175 Literature cited [173] Sukhdeo, M. V.K. 2012. Where are the parasites in food webs? Parasites and Vectors 5:239. [174] Fontaine, C. et al. 2011. The ecological and evolutionary implications of merging different types of networks. Ecol. Lett. 14:1170–1181. [175] Ulanowicz, R. E. et al. 2014. Limits on ecosystem trophic complexity: insights from ecological network analysis. Ecol. Lett. 17:127–136. [176] Kleiber, M. 1932. Body size and metabolism. Hilgardia 6(11):315–353. [177] Brown, J.H. et al. 2004. Toward a metabolic theory of ecology. Ecology 85(7):1771–1789. [178] Calder III, W.A. 1983. Ecological scaling: mammals and birds. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Syst. 14:213–230. [179] Pomeroy, D. 1990. Why fly? The possible benefits for lower mortality. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 40:53–65. [180] Arditi, R. & Ginzburg, L.R. 1989. Coupling in predator-prey dynamics: ratio-depen- dence. J. Theor. Biol. 139:311–326. [181] Akçakaya, H.R. et al. 1995. Ratio-dependent predation: an abstraction that works. Ecology 76(3):995–1004. [182] Abrams, P. A. & Ginzburg, L.R. 2000. The nature of predation: prey dependent, ratio dependent or neither? Trends Ecol. Evol. 15(8):337–341. [183] Arditi, R. & Ginzburg, L.R. 2012. How Species Interact: Altering the Standard View on Trophic Ecology. Oxford University Press, New York. [184] Piana, P. A. et al. 2006. Comparison of predator-prey interaction models for fish assemblages from the neotropical region. Ecol. Model. 192:259–270. [185] Kratina, P. et al. 2009. Functional responses modified by predator density. Oecologia 159:425–433. [186] Berg, H. A. van den 1998. Propagation of permanent perturbations in food chains and food webs. Ecol. Model. 107:225–235. [187] Novak, M. 2013. Trophic omnivory across a productivity gradient: intraguild preda- tion theory and the structure and strength of species interactions. Proc. R. Soc. B 280:20131415. [188] Preston, D.L. et al. 2013. Biomass and productivity of trematode parasites in pond ecosystems. J. Anim. Ecol. 82:509–517. [189] Staniczenko, P. P. A. et al. 2010. Structural dynamics and robustness of food webs. Ecol. Lett. 13:891–899. [190] Valdovinos, F. S. et al. 2010. Consequences of adaptive behaviour for the structure and dynamics of food webs. Ecol. Lett. 13:1546–1559. [191] Thierry, A. et al. 2011. Adaptive foraging and the rewiring of size-structured food webs following extinctions. Basic Appl. Ecol. 12:562–570. 176 Literature cited [192] Ramos-Jiliberto, R. et al. 2012. Topological plasticity increases robustness of mutu- alistic networks. J. Anim. Ecol. 81:896–904. [193] Cohen, A. E. et al. 1998. A novel experimental apparatus to study the impact of white noise and 1/f noise on animal populations. Proc. R. Soc. B 265:11–15. [194] Cohen, J. E. et al. 1999. Spectral mimicry: a method of synthesizing matching time series with different fourier spectra. Circuits Syst. Signal Process. 18(3):431–442. [195] Fowler, M. S. & Ruokolainen, L. 2013. Confounding environmental colour and dis- tribution shape leads to underestimation of population extinction risk. PLoS ONE 8(2):e55855. [196] Roughgarden, J. 1975. A simple model for population dynamics in stochastic environ- ments. Am. Nat. 109(970):713–736. [197] Sæther, B.-E. et al. 1998. Environmental stochasticity and extinction risk in a popu- lation of a small songbird, the Great Tit. Am. Nat. 151(5):441–450. [198] Neubert, M.G. et al. 2004. Reactivity and transient dynamics of predator-prey and food web models. Ecol. Model. 179:29–38. [199] Ruokolainen, L. et al. 2009. Ecological and evolutionary dynamics under coloured environmental variation. Trends Ecol. Evol. 24(10):555–563. [200] Sutherland, W. J. et al. 2013. Identification of 100 fundamental ecological questions. J. Ecol. 101:58–67. [201] Solé, R. V. et al. 2002. Self-organized instability in complex ecosystems. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 357:667–681. [202] Ruokolainen, L. & Fowler, M. S. 2008. Community extinction patterns in coloured environments. Proc. R. Soc. B 275:1775–1783. [203] Levins, R. 1969. The effect of random variations of different types on population growth. P. Natl. Acad. Sci. usa 62:1061–1065. [204] Lewontin, R. C. & Cohen, D. 1969. On population growth in a randomly varying environment. P. Natl. Acad. Sci. usa 62:1056–1060. [205] Dennis, B. 2002. Allee effects in stochastic populations. Oikos 96(3):389–401. [206] Greenman, J. V. & Benton, T.G. 2005. The impact of environmental fluctuations on structured discrete time population models: resonance, synchrony and threshold behaviour. Theor. Pop. Biol. 68:217–235. [207] Ebenman, B. & Jonsson, T. 2005. Using community viability analysis to identify fragile systems and keystone species. Trends Ecol. Evol. 20(10):568–575. [208] Ripa, J. et al. 1998. A general theory of environmental noise in ecological food webs. Am. Nat. 151(3):256–263. [209] Spagnolo, B. & La Barbera, A. 2002. Role of the noise on the transient dynamics of an ecosystem of interacting species. Physica A 315:114–124. 177 Literature cited [210] Ripa, J. & Ives, A.R. 2003. Food web dynamics in correlated and autocorrelated environments. Theor. Pop. Biol. 64:369–384. [211] Gravel, D. et al. 2011. Species coexistence in a variable world. Ecol. Lett. 14:828–839. [212] Gjata, N. et al. 2012. The strength of simulated indirect interaction modules in a real food web. Ecol. Complex. 11:160–164. [213] Vasseur, D. A. & Fox, J.W. 2007. Environmental fluctuations can stabilize food web dynamics by increasing synchrony. Ecol. Lett. 10:1066–1074. [214] Steele, J.H. 1985. A comparison of terrestrial and marine ecological systems. Nature 313:355–358. [215] Pimm, S. L. & Redfearn, A. 1988. The variability of population densities. Nature 334:613–614. [216] Halley, J.M. 1996. Ecology, evolution and )=f-noise. Trends Ecol. Evol. 11(1):33–37. [217] Morales, J.M. 1999. Viability in a pink environment: why “white noise” models can be dangerous. Ecol. Lett. 2:228–232. [218] Halley, J.M. & Inchausti, P. 2004. The increasing importance of 1/f-noises as models of ecological variability. Fluct. Noise Lett. 4(2):r1–r26. [219] Vasseur, D. A. & Yodzis, P. 2004. The color of environmental noise. Ecology 85(4):1146– 1152. [220] Hennig, H. 2014. Synchronization in human musical rhythms and mutually interacting complex systems. P. Natl. Acad. Sci. usa 111(36):12974–12979. [221] Petchey, O.L. et al. 1997. Effects on population persistence: the interaction be- tween environmental noise colour, intraspecific competition and space. Proc. R. Soc. B 264:1841–1847. [222] Cuddington, K.M. & Yodzis, P. 1999. Black noise and population persistence. Proc. R. Soc. B 266:969–973. [223] Ripa, J. & Heino, M. 1999. Linear analysis solves two puzzles in population dynamics: the route to extinction and extinction in coloured environments. Ecol. Lett. 2:219–222. [224] Greenman, J. V. & Benton, T.G. 2003. The amplification of environmental noise in population models: causes and consequences. Am. Nat. 161(2):225–239. [225] Reuman, D.C. et al. 2006. Power spectra reveal the influence of stochasticity on nonlinear population dynamics. P. Natl. Acad. Sci. usa 103(49):18860–18865. [226] Reuman, D.C. et al. 2008. Colour of environmental noise affects the nonlinear dy- namics of cycling, stage-structured populations. Ecol. Lett. 11:820–830. [227] Ezard, T.H.G. & Coulson, T. 2010. How sensitive are elasticities of long-run stochas- tic growth to how environmental variability is modelled? Ecol. Model. 221:191–200. [228] Lögdberg, F. & Wennergren, U. 2012. Spectral color, synchrony, and extinction risk. Theor. Ecol. 5(4):545–554. 178 Literature cited [229] Vasseur, D. A. 2007. Populations embedded in trophic communities respond differ- ently to coloured environmental noise. Theor. Pop. Biol. 72:186–196. [230] Ruokolainen, L. et al. 2007. Extinctions in competitive communities forced by coloured environmental variation. Oikos 116:439–448. [231] Loreau, M. & Mazancourt, C. de 2008. Species synchrony and its drivers: neutral and nonneutral community dynamics in fluctuating environments. Am. Nat. 172(2):e48–e66. [232] Moran, P. A. P. 1953. The statistical analysis of the Canadian lynx cycle. Australian J. Zool. 1(3):291–298. [233] Raimondo, S. et al. 2004. Population synchrony within and among Lepidoptera species in relation to weather, phylogeny, and larval phenology. Ecol. Entomol. 29:96–105. [234] Bowman, J. et al. 2008. Spatial and temporal dynamics of small mammals at a regional scale in Canadian boreal forest. J. Mammal. 89(2):381–387. [235] Kvasnes, M. A. J. et al. 2010. Spatial dynamics of Norwegian tetraonid populations. Ecol. Res. 25:367–374. [236] Tachiki, Y. et al. 2010. Pollinator coupling can induce synchronized flowering in different plant species. J. Theor. Biol. 267:153–163. [237] Chevalier, M. et al. 2014. Spatial synchrony in stream fish populations: influence of species traits. Ecography 37(10):960–968. [238] Yachi, S. & Loreau, M. 1999. Biodiversity and ecosystem productivity in a fluctuating environment: the insurance hypothesis. P. Natl. Acad. Sci. usa 96:1463–1468. [239] Morin, X. et al. 2014. Temporal stability in forest productivity increases with tree diversity due to asynchrony in species dynamics. Ecol. Lett. 17:1526–1535. [240] Borrvall, C. & Ebenman, B. 2008. Biodiversity and persistence of ecological commu- nities in variable environments. Ecol. Complex. 5:99–105. [241] Loreau, M. & Mazancourt, C. de 2013. Biodiversity and ecosystem stability: a syn- thesis of underlying mechanisms. Ecol. Lett. 16:106–115. [242] Kokkoris, G.D. et al. 2002. Variability in interaction strength and implications for biodiversity. J. Anim. Ecol. 71:362–371. [243] Allesina, S. & Levine, J.M. 2011. A competitive network theory of species diversity. P. Natl. Acad. Sci. usa 108(14):5638–5642. [244] Bruno, J. F. et al. 2003. Inclusion of facilitation into ecological theory. Trends Ecol. Evol. 18(3):119–125. [245] Xie, J. et al. 2011. Ecological mechanisms underlying the sustainability of the agricul- tural heritage rice-fish coculture system. P. Natl. Acad. Sci. usa 108(50):e1381–e1387. [246] He, Q. et al. 2013. Global shifts towards positive species interactions with increasing environmental stress. Ecol. Lett. 16:695–706. [247] Okuyama, T. & Holland, J.N. 2008. Network structural properties mediate the sta- bility of mutualistic communities. Ecol. Lett. 11:208–216. 179 Literature cited [248] Kondoh, M. et al. 2010. Food webs are built up with nested subwebs. Ecology 91(11):3123– 3130. [249] Thébault, E. & Fontaine, C. 2010. Stability of ecological communities and the architecture of mutualistic and trophic networks. Science 329:853–856. [250] Bascompte, J. 2010. Structure and dynamics of ecological networks. Science 329:765– 766. [251] Goudard, A. & Loreau, M. 2008. Nontrophic interactions, biodiversity, and ecosystem functioning: an interaction web model. Am. Nat. 171(1):91–106. [252] Visser, S.N. de et al. 2011. The Serengeti food web: empirical quantification and analysis of topological changes under increasing human impact. J. Anim. Ecol. 80:484– 494. [253] Orlofske, S. A. et al. 2012. Parasite transmission in complex communities: predators and alternative hosts alter pathogenic infections in amphibians. Ecology 93(6):1247–1253. [254] Filotas, E. et al. 2010. Positive interactions and the emergence of community structure in metacommunities. J. Theor. Biol. 266:419–429. [255] Lee, K.-M. et al. 2014. Multiplex networks. In: D’Agostino, G. & Scala, A., eds. Networks of Networks: The Last Frontier of Complexity. Springer pp. 53–72. [256] Sauve, A.M.C. et al. 2014. Structure-stability relationships in networks combining mutualistic and antagonistic interactions. Oikos 123:378–384. [257] Yoshikawa, T. & Isagi, Y. 2014. Determination of temperate bird-flower interactions as entangled mutualistic and antagonistic sub-networks: characterization at the net- work and species levels. J. Anim. Ecol. 83:651–660. [258] Scheffer, M. et al. 2001. Catastrophic shifts in ecosystems. Nature 413:591–596. [259] Folke, C. et al. 2004. Regime shifts, resilience, and biodiversity in ecosystem man- agement. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Evol. Syst. 35:557–581. [260] Marcogliese, D. J. & Cone, D.K. 1997. Food webs: a plea for parasites. Trends Ecol. Evol. 12(8):320–325. [261] Wood, M. J. 2006. Parasites entangled in food webs. Trends Parasitol. 23(1):8–10. [262] Lewis, O.T. et al. 2002. Structure of a diverse tropical forest insect-parasitoid com- munity. J. Anim. Ecol. 71:855–873. [263] Maunsell, S. C. et al. 2014. Changes in host-parasitoid food web structure with ele- vation. J. Anim. Ecol. 84(2):353–363. [264] Byers, J. E. 2008. Including parasites in food webs. Trends Parasitol. 25(2):55–57. [265] Chen, H.-W. et al. 2011. The reduction of food web robustness by parasitism: fact and artefact. Int. J. Parasitol. 41:627–634. [266] Sato, T. et al. 2012. Nematomorph parasites indirectly alter the food web and ecosys- tem function of streams through behavioural manipulation of their cricket hosts. Ecol. Lett. 15:786–793. 180 Literature cited [267] Dunne, J. A. et al. 2013. Parasites affect food web structure primarily through increased diversity and complexity. PLoS Biol. 11(6):e1001579. [268] Holland, J.N. et al. 2013. Consumer-resource dynamics of indirect interactions in a mutualism-parasitism food web module. Theor. Ecol. 6:475–493. [269] Stouffer, D. B. et al. 2011. The role of body mass in diet contiguity and food-web structure. J. Anim. Ecol. 80:632–639. [270] Zook, A. E. et al. 2011. Food webs: ordering species according to body size yields high degree of intervality. J. Theor. Biol. 271:106–113. [271] Digel, C. et al. 2011. Body sizes, cumulative and allometric degree distributions across natural food webs. Oikos 120:503–509. [272] Lehman, C. L. & Tilman, D. 2000. Biodiversity, stability, and productivity in com- petitive communities. Am. Nat. 156(5):534–552. [273] Jost, L. 2006. Entropy and diversity. Oikos 113(2):363–375. [274] Ellis, M.M. & Crone, E. E. 2013. The role of transient dynamics in stochastic pop- ulation growth for nine perennial plants. Ecology 94(8):1681–1686. [275] Ripley, B. 2013. tree: Classification and regression trees. http: / /CRAN.R- project.org/ package=tree. [276] Andersen, K.H. et al. 2009. Trophic and individual efficiencies of size-structured communities. Proc. R. Soc. B 276:109–114. [277] Scheffer, M. et al. 2012. Anticipating critical transitions. Science 338:344–348. [278] Thébault, E. & Loreau, M. 2003. Food-web constraints on biodiversity–ecosystem functioning relationships. P. Natl. Acad. Sci. usa 100(25):14949–14954. [279] 2005. Trophic interactions and the relationship between species diversity and ecosystem stability. Am. Nat. 166(4):e95–e114. [280] 2006. The relationship between biodiversity and ecosystem functioning in food webs. Ecol. Res. 21:17–25. [281] Mougi, A. & Kondoh, M. 2012. Diversity of interaction types and ecological commu- nity stability. Science 337:349–351. [282] Otto, S. B. et al. 2007. Allometric degree distributions facilitate food-web stability. Nature 450:1226–1229. [283] Walker, B. 1995. Conserving biological diversity through ecosystem resilience. Conserv. Biol. 9(4):747–752. [284] Elmqvist, T. et al. 2003. Response diversity, ecosystem change, and resilience. Front. Ecol. Environ. 1(9):488–494. [285] Stouffer, D. B. et al. 2012. Evolutionary conservation of species’ roles in food webs. Science 335:1489–1492. 181 Literature cited [286] Hooper, D.U. et al. 2012. A global synthesis reveals biodiversity loss as a major driver of ecosystem change. Nature 486:105–108. [287] Tylianakis, J.M. et al. 2008. Global change and species interactions in terrestrial ecosystems. Ecol. Lett. 11:1351–1363. [288] Curtsdotter, A. et al. 2011. Robustness to secondary extinctions: comparing trait- based sequential deletions in static and dynamic food webs. Basic Appl. Ecol. 12:571– 580. [289] Sahasrabudhe, S. & Motter, A. E. 2011. Rescuing ecosystems from extinction cascades through compensatory perturbations. Nat. Commun. 2:170. [290] Stouffer, D. B. & Bascompte, J. 2011. Compartmentalization increases food-web persistence. P. Natl. Acad. Sci. usa 108(9):3648–3652. [291] Schneider, F.D. et al. 2012. Body mass constraints on feeding rates determine the consequences of predator loss. Ecol. Lett. 15:436–443. [292] Säterberg, T. et al. 2013. High frequency of functional extinctions in ecological networks. Nature 499:468–470. [293] Riede, J.O. et al. 2011. Size-based food web characteristics govern the response to species extinctions. Basic Appl. Ecol. 12:581–589. [294] Torres-Alruiz, M.D. & Rodríguez, D. J. 2013. A topo-dynamical perspective to evaluate indirect interactions in trophic webs: new indexes. Ecol. Model. 250:363–369. [295] Valiente-Banuet, A. & Verdú, M. 2013. Human impacts on multiple ecological net- works act synergistically to drive ecosystem collapse. Front. Ecol. Environ. 11(8):408– 413. [296] Woodward, G. & Hildrew, A.G. 2001. Invasion of a stream food web by a new top predator. J. Anim. Ecol. 70:273–288. [297] Layer, K. et al. 2011. Long-term dynamics of a well-characterised food web: four decades of acidification and recovery in the Broadstone Stream model system. Adv. Ecol. Res. 44:69–117. [298] Galiana, N. et al. 2014. Invasions cause biodiversity loss and community simplification in vertebrate food webs. Oikos 123:721–728. [299] Traveset, A. & Richardson, D.M. 2006. Biol. Invasions as disruptors of plant repro- ductive mutualisms. Trends Ecol. Evol. 21(4):208–216. [300] Memmott, J. 2009. Food webs: a ladder for picking strawberries or a practical tool for practical problems? Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 364:1693–1699. [301] Burkle, L. A. & Alarcón, R. 2011. The future of plant-pollinator diversity: under- standing interaction networks across time, space, and global change. Am. J. Bot. 98(3):528–538. [302] Rodriguez-Cabal, M. A. et al. 2013. Node-by-node disassembly of a mutualistic in- teraction web driven by species introductions. P. Natl. Acad. Sci. usa 110(41):16503– 16507. 182 Literature cited [303] Traveset, A. et al. 2013. Invaders of pollination networks in the Galápagos Islands: emergence of novel communities. Proc. R. Soc. B 280:20123040. [304] Stouffer, D. B. et al. 2014. How exotic plants integrate into pollination networks. J. Ecol. 102:1442–1450. [305] Sugiura, S. 2010. Species interactions-area relationships: biological invasions and network structure in relation to island area. Proc. R. Soc. B 277:1807–1815. [306] Vacher, C. et al. 2010. Ecological integration of alien species into a tree-parasitic fungus network. Biol. Invasions 12:3249–3259. [307] Strayer, D. L. 2012. Eight questions about invasions and ecosystem functioning. Ecol. Lett. 15:1199–1210. [308] Gurevitch, J. & Padilla, D.K. 2004. Are invasive species a major cause of extinctions? Trends Ecol. Evol. 19(9):470–474. [309] Wardle, D. A. et al. 2011. Terrestrial ecosystem responses to species gains and losses. Science 332:1273–1277. [310] Forys, E. A. & Allen, C.R. 2002. Functional group change within and across scales following invasions and extinctions in the Everglades ecosystem. Ecosystems 5:339–347. [311] Jackson, S. T. & Sax, D. F. 2010. Balancing biodiversity in a changing environment: extinction debt, immigration credit and species turnover. Trends Ecol. Evol. 25(3):153– 160. [312] Cucherousset, J. et al. 2012. Non-native species promote trophic dispersion of food webs. Front. Ecol. Environ. 10:406–408. [313] Estes, J. A. et al. 2011. Trophic downgrading of Planet Earth. Science 333:301–306. [314] Kardol, P. & Wardle, D. A. 2010. How understanding aboveground-belowground linkages can assist restoration ecology. Trends Ecol. Evol. 25(11):670–679. [315] Lundberg, P. et al. 2000. Species loss leads to community closure. Ecol. Lett. 3:465–468. [316] Zavaleta, E. S. et al. 2001. Viewing invasive species removal in a whole-ecosystem context. Trends Ecol. Evol. 16(8):454–459. [317] Carvalheiro, L.G. et al. 2008. Pollinator networks, alien species and the conservation of rare plants: Trinia glauca as a case study. J. Appl. Ecol. 45:1419–1427. [318] Raymond, B. et al. 2011. Qualitative modelling of invasive species eradication on subantarctic Macquarie Island. J. Appl. Ecol. 48:181–191. [319] Cardinale, B. 2012. Impacts of biodiversity loss. Science 336:552–553. [320] Hulme, P. E. et al. 2013. Bias and error in understanding plant invasion impacts. Trends Ecol. Evol. 28(4):212–218. [321] Donohue, I. et al. 2013. On the dimensionality of ecological stability. Ecol. Lett. 16:421– 429. 183 Literature cited [322] Joo, J. et al. 2007. Effects of noise on ecological invasion processes: bacteriophage- mediated competition in bacteria. J. Stat. Phys. 128:229–256. [323] Simberloff, D. 2013. Biol. Invasions: what’s worth fighting and what can be won? Ecol. Eng. 65:112–121. [324] Sobol’, I.M. 1967. On the distribution of points in a cube and the approximate eval- uation of integrals. USSR Computational Mathematics and Mathematical Physics 7(4):86– 112. [325] Greenman, J. V. & Benton, T.G. 2005. The frequency spectrum of structured discrete time population models: its properties and their ecological implications. Oikos 110:369– 389. [326] O’Neill, R. V. 2001. Is it time to bury the ecosystem concept? Ecology 82(12):3275–3284. [327] Zaccarelli, N. et al. 2013. Order and disorder in ecological time-series: introducing normalized spectral entropy. Ecol. Indic. 28:22–30. [328] Gross, N. et al. 2013. Functional differences between alien and native species: do biotic interactions determine the functional structure of highly invaded grasslands? Funct. Ecol. 27(5):1262–1272. [329] Gallardo, B. & Aldridge, D.C. 2013. Priority setting for invasive species manage- ment: risk assessment of Ponto-Caspian invasive species into Great Britain. Ecol. Appl. 23(2):352–364. [330] Franklin, J. et al. 2008. Evaluating extreme risks in invasion ecology: learning from banking compliance. Divers. Distrib. 14:581–591. [331] Burgman, M.A. et al. 2010. Reconciling uncertain costs and benefits in Bayes nets for invasive species management. Risk Anal. 30(2):277–284. [332] Vilcinskas, A. et al. 2013. Invasive harlequin ladybird carries biological weapons against native competitors. Science 340:862–863. [333] Hobbs, R. J. et al. 2009. Novel ecosystems: implications for conservation and restora- tion. Trends Ecol. Evol. 24(11):599–605. [334] Larson, B.M.H. et al. 2013. Managing invasive species amidst high uncertainty and novelty. Trends Ecol. Evol. 28(5):255–256. [335] Simberloff, D. et al. 2013. Impacts of Biol. Invasions: what’s what and the way forward. Trends Ecol. Evol. 28(1):58–66. [336] Yelenik, S.G. & D’Antonio, C.M. 2013. Self-reinforcing impacts of plant invasions change over time. Nature 503:517–520. [337] Ripple, W. J. & Beschta, R. L. 2004. Wolves and the ecology of fear: can predation risk structure ecosystems? BioScience 54(8):755–766. [338] Nicholls, H. 2013. The 18-km2 rat trap. Nature 497:306–308. [339] Lampert, A. et al. 2014. Optimal approaches for balancing invasive species eradication and endangered species management. Science 344:1028–1031. 184 Literature cited [340] Russell, J. C. et al. 2014. Over-invasion by functionally equivalent invasive species. Ecology 95(8):2268–2276. [341] Poisot, T. et al. 2013. Trophic complementarity drives the biodiversity-ecosystem functioning relationship in food webs. Ecol. Lett. 16:853–861. [342] O’Connor, N.E. & Donohue, I. 2013. Environmental context determines multi- trophic effects of consumer species loss. Glob. Change Biol. 19:431–440. [343] Cornelius, S. P. et al. 2013. Realistic control of network dynamics. Nat. Commun. 4:1942. [344] Isbell, F. & Loreau, M. 2013. Human impacts on minimum subsets of species critical for maintaining ecosystem structure. Basic Appl. Ecol. 14:623–629. [345] Ghedini, C.G. & Ribeiro, C.H.C. 2011. Rethinking failure and attack tolerance assessment in complex networks. Physica A 390:4684–4691. [346] Mones, E. et al. 2014. Shock waves on complex networks. Sci. Rep. 4:4949. [347] Cowan, N. J. et al. 2012. Nodal dynamics, not degree distributions, determine the structural controllability of complex networks. PLoS ONE 7(6):e38398. [348] Quax, R. et al. 2013. The diminishing role of hubs in dynamical processes on complex networks. J. R. Soc. Interface 10:20130568. [349] Galbiati, M. et al. 2013. The power to control. Nat. Phys. 9:126–128. [350] Wuchty, S. 2014. Controllability in protein interaction networks. P. Natl. Acad. Sci. usa 111(19):7156–7160. [351] Hsieh, H.-Y. & Perfecto, I. 2012. Trait-mediated indirect effects of phorid flies on ants. Psyche 2012:380474. [352] Kamran-Disfani, A.R. & Golubski, A. J. 2013. Lateral cascade of indirect effects in food webs with different types of adaptive behavior. J. Theor. Biol. 339:58–69. [353] Wootton, J. T. 1993. Indirect effects and habitat use in an intertidal community: interaction chains and interaction modifications. Am. Nat. 141(1):71–89. [354] 1994. The nature and consequences of indirect effects in ecological commu- nities. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Syst. 25:443–466. [355] Dambacher, J.M. & Ramos-Jiliberto, R. 2007. Understanding and predicting effects of modified interactions through a qualitative analysis of community structure. Q. Rev. Biol. 82(3):227–250. [356] Werner, E. E. & Peacor, S.D. 2003. A review of trait-mediated indirect interactions in ecological communities. Ecology 84(5):1083–1100. [357] Krenek, L. & Rudolf, V.H.W. 2014. Allometric scaling of indirect effects: body size ratios predict non-consumptive effects in multi-predator systems. J. Anim. Ecol. 83:1461–1468. [358] Arditi, R. et al. 2005. Rheagogies: modelling non-trophic effects in food webs. Ecol. Complex. 2:249–258. 185 Literature cited [359] Ulanowicz, R. E. & Puccia, C. J. 1990. Mixed trophic impacts in ecosystems. Coenoses 5(1):7–16. [360] Schmitz, O. et al. 2004. Trophic cascades: the primacy of trait-mediated indirect interactions. Ecol. Lett. 7:153–163. [361] Bolker, B. et al. 2003. Connecting theoretical and empirical studies of trait-mediated interactions. Ecology 84(5):1101–1114. [362] Hsieh, H.-Y. et al. 2012. Cascading trait-mediated interactions induced by ant phero- mones. Ecol. Evol. 2(9):2181–2191. [363] Roode, J. C. de et al. 2011. Aphids indirectly increase virulence and transmission potential of a monarch butterfly parasite by reducing defensive chemistry of a shared food plant. Ecol. Lett. 14:453–461. [364] Abrams, P. A. 1995. Implications of dynamically variable traits for identifying, classi- fying, and measuring direct and indirect effects in ecological communities. Am. Nat. 146(1):112–134. [365] Sanders, D. et al. 2014. Integrating ecosystem engineering and food webs. Oikos 123(5):513–524. [366] Helbing, D. 2013. Globally networked risks and how to respond. Nature 497:51–59. [367] D’Agostino, G. & Scala, A., eds. 2014. Networks of Networks: The Last Frontier of Complexity. Springer. [368] Kivelä, M. et al. 2014. Multilayer networks. J. Complex Netw. 2:203–271. [369] Gao, J. et al. 2012. Networks formed from interdependent networks. Nat. Phys. 8:40– 48. [370] Newman, M.E. J. 2012. Communities, modules and large-scale structure in networks. Nat. Phys. 8:25–31. [371] Flake, G.W. et al. 2002. Self-organization and identification of web communities. ieee Comput. 35:66–71. [372] Betzel, R. F. et al. 2013. Multi-scale community organization of the human structural connectome and its relationship with resting-state functional connectivity. Network Science 1(3):353–373. [373] Hartwell, L.H. et al. 1999. From molecular to modular cell biology. Nature 402:c47– c52. [374] Wagner, A. & Fell, D. A. 2001. The small world inside large metabolic networks. Proc. R. Soc. B 268:1803–1810. [375] Krause, A. E. et al. 2003. Compartments revealed in food-web structure. Nature 426:282– 285. [376] Variano, E. A. et al. 2004. Networks, dynamics, and modularity. Phys. Rev. Lett. 92(18):188701. 186 Literature cited [377] Domenico, M. de et al. 2014. Navigability of interconnected networks under random failures. P. Natl. Acad. Sci. usa 111(23):8351–8356. [378] Brummitt, C.D. et al. 2012. Suppressing cascades of load in interdependent networks. P. Natl. Acad. Sci. usa 109(12):e680–e689. [379] Rigterink, D. & Singer, D. J. 2014. A network complexity metric based on planarity and community structure. J. Complex Netw. 2:299–312. [380] Pimm, S. L. & Lawton, J.H. 1980. Are food webs divided into compartments? J. Anim. Ecol. 49:879–898. [381] Montoya, J.M. et al. 2006. Ecological networks and their fragility. Nature 442:259– 264. [382] Raffaelli, D. & Hall, S. J. 1992. Compartments and predation in an estuarine food web. J. Anim. Ecol. 61:551–560. [383] Fonseca, C.R. & Ganade, G. 1996. Asymmetries, compartments and null interactions in an Amazonian ant-plant community. J. Anim. Ecol. 65:339–347. [384] Prado, P. I. & Lewinsohn, T.M. 2004. Compartments in insect-plant associations and their consequences for community structure. J. Anim. Ecol. 73:1168–1178. [385] Hines, J. & Gessner, M.O. 2012. Consumer trophic diversity as a fundamental mech- anism linking predation and ecosystem functioning. J. Anim. Ecol. 81:1146–1153. [386] Memmott, J. et al. 1994. The structure of a tropical host-parasitoid community. J. Anim. Ecol. 63:521–540. [387] Giery, S. T. et al. 2013. Bidirectional trophic linkages couple canopy and understorey food webs. Funct. Ecol. 27:1436–1441. [388] Bellmore, J. R. et al. 2013. The floodplain food web mosaic: a study of its importance to salmon and steelhead with implications for their recovery. Ecol. Appl. 23(1):189–207. [389] Nummi, P. et al. 2011. Bats benefit from beavers: a facilitative link between aquatic and terrestrial food webs. Biodivers. Conserv. 20:851–859. [390] Evans, D.M. et al. 2013. The robustness of a network of ecological networks to habitat loss. Ecol. Lett. 16:844–852. [391] Allesina, S. & Pascual, M. 2009. Food web models: a plea for groups. Ecol. Lett. 12:652–662. [392] Digel, C. et al. 2014. Unravelling the complex structure of forest soil food webs: higher omnivory and more trophic levels. Oikos 123:1157–1172. [393] Csardi, G. & Nepusz, T. 2006. The igraph software package for complex network research. InterJournal Complex Systems:1695. [394] Crooks, K.R. & Soulé, M.E. 1999. Mesopredator release and avifaunal extinctions in a fragmented system. Nature 400:563–566. [395] Mouquet, N. et al. 2012. Extending the concept of keystone species to communities and ecosystems. Ecol. Lett. 16:1–8. 187 Literature cited [396] Albrecht, M. et al. 2014. Consequences of plant invasions on compartmentalization and species’ roles in plant-pollinator networks. Proc. R. Soc. B 281:20140773. [397] Gopalan, P. K. & Blei, D.M. 2013. Efficient discovery of overlapping communities in massive networks. P. Natl. Acad. Sci. usa 110(36):14534–14539. [398] Woodroffe, R. & Ginsberg, J. R. 1998. Edge effects and the extinction of populations inside protected areas. Science 280:2126–2128. [399] Huxel, G.R. et al. 2004. At the frontier of the integration of food web ecology and landscape ecology. In: Polis, G. A. et al., eds. Food Webs at the Landscape Level. University of Chicago Press, Chicago pp. 434–451. [400] Polis, G. A. et al., eds. 2004. Food Webs at the Landscape Level. University of Chicago Press, Chicago. [401] Palmer, M.A. & Febria, C.M. 2012. The heartbeat of ecosystems. Science 336:1393– 1394. [402] Tylianakis, J.M. et al. 2010. Conservation of species interaction networks. Biol. Conserv. 143:2270–2279. [403] Norris, K. 2012. Biodiversity in the context of ecosystem services: the applied need for systems approaches. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 367:191–199. [404] Rodewald, A.D. et al. 2014. Community-level demographic consequences of urban- ization: an ecological network approach. J. Anim. Ecol. 83:1409–1417. [405] Sutherland, W. J. et al. 2004. The need for evidence-based conservation. Trends Ecol. Evol. 19(6):305–308. [406] Eklöf, A. et al. 2012. Relevance of evolutionary history for food web structure. Proc. R. Soc. B 279:1588–1596. [407] Naisbit, R. E. et al. 2012. Phylogeny versus body size as determinants of food web structure. Proc. R. Soc. B 282:20120327. [408] Chamberlain, S. et al. 2014. Phylogenetic tree shape and the structure of mutualistic networks. J. Ecol. 102:1234–1243. [409] Minoarivelo, H.O. et al. 2014. Detecting phylogenetic signal in mutualistic interac- tion networks using a Markov process model. Oikos 123:1250–1260. [410] Yan, C. & Zhang, Z. 2014. Specific non-monotonous interactions increase persistence of ecological networks. Proc. R. Soc. B 281:20132797. [411] Moya-Laraño, J. et al. 2012. Climate change and eco-evolutionary dynamics in food webs. Adv. Ecol. Res. 47:1–80. [412] Fortuna, M. A. et al. 2013. Evolving digital ecological networks. PLoS Comput. Biol. 9(3):e1002928. [413] Held, I. 2014. Simplicity amid complexity. Science 343:1206–1207. [414] Tero, A. et al. 2010. Rules for biologically inspired adaptive network design. Science 327:439–442. 188 Literature cited [415] Sutherland, W. J. & Freckleton, R. P. 2012. Making predictive ecology more relevant to policy makers and practitioners. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 367:322–330. [416] Milner-Gulland, E. J. 2012. Interactions between human behaviour and ecological systems. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 367:270–278. [417] Horan, R.D. et al. 2011. Managing ecological thresholds in coupled environmental- human systems. P. Natl. Acad. Sci. usa 108(18):7333–7338. [418] Soltani, A. et al. 2014. A dynamic bio-economic model for community management of goat and oak forests in Zagros, Iran. Ecol. Econ. 106:174–185. [419] Parrott, L. & Meyer, W. S. 2012. Future landscapes: managing within complexity. Front. Ecol. Environ. 10(7):382–389. [420] Filotas, E. et al. 2014. Viewing forests through the lens of complex systems science. Ecosphere 5(1):1. [421] Sutherland, W. J. & Woodroof, H. J. 2009. The need for environmental horizon scanning. Trends Ecol. Evol. 24(10):523–527. [422] Sutherland, W. J. et al. 2014. A horizon scan of global conservation issues for 2014. Trends Ecol. Evol. 29(1):15–22. [423] Ben-Haim, Y. 2006. Info-Gap Decision Theory: Decisions under Severe Uncertainty. Aca- demic Press, Oxford. [424] Helbing, D. et al. 2014. Saving human lives: what complexity science and information systems can contribute. J. Stat. Phys. 158(3):735–781. [425] Loreau, M. 2010. From Populations to Ecosystems: Theoretical Foundations for a New Ecolog- ical Synthesis. Monographs in Population Biology 46. Princeton University Press, New Jersey. 189